• Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Problem-Solving Strategies and Obstacles

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Sean is a fact-checker and researcher with experience in sociology, field research, and data analytics.

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

JGI / Jamie Grill / Getty Images

  • Application
  • Improvement

From deciding what to eat for dinner to considering whether it's the right time to buy a house, problem-solving is a large part of our daily lives. Learn some of the problem-solving strategies that exist and how to use them in real life, along with ways to overcome obstacles that are making it harder to resolve the issues you face.

What Is Problem-Solving?

In cognitive psychology , the term 'problem-solving' refers to the mental process that people go through to discover, analyze, and solve problems.

A problem exists when there is a goal that we want to achieve but the process by which we will achieve it is not obvious to us. Put another way, there is something that we want to occur in our life, yet we are not immediately certain how to make it happen.

Maybe you want a better relationship with your spouse or another family member but you're not sure how to improve it. Or you want to start a business but are unsure what steps to take. Problem-solving helps you figure out how to achieve these desires.

The problem-solving process involves:

  • Discovery of the problem
  • Deciding to tackle the issue
  • Seeking to understand the problem more fully
  • Researching available options or solutions
  • Taking action to resolve the issue

Before problem-solving can occur, it is important to first understand the exact nature of the problem itself. If your understanding of the issue is faulty, your attempts to resolve it will also be incorrect or flawed.

Problem-Solving Mental Processes

Several mental processes are at work during problem-solving. Among them are:

  • Perceptually recognizing the problem
  • Representing the problem in memory
  • Considering relevant information that applies to the problem
  • Identifying different aspects of the problem
  • Labeling and describing the problem

Problem-Solving Strategies

There are many ways to go about solving a problem. Some of these strategies might be used on their own, or you may decide to employ multiple approaches when working to figure out and fix a problem.

An algorithm is a step-by-step procedure that, by following certain "rules" produces a solution. Algorithms are commonly used in mathematics to solve division or multiplication problems. But they can be used in other fields as well.

In psychology, algorithms can be used to help identify individuals with a greater risk of mental health issues. For instance, research suggests that certain algorithms might help us recognize children with an elevated risk of suicide or self-harm.

One benefit of algorithms is that they guarantee an accurate answer. However, they aren't always the best approach to problem-solving, in part because detecting patterns can be incredibly time-consuming.

There are also concerns when machine learning is involved—also known as artificial intelligence (AI)—such as whether they can accurately predict human behaviors.

Heuristics are shortcut strategies that people can use to solve a problem at hand. These "rule of thumb" approaches allow you to simplify complex problems, reducing the total number of possible solutions to a more manageable set.

If you find yourself sitting in a traffic jam, for example, you may quickly consider other routes, taking one to get moving once again. When shopping for a new car, you might think back to a prior experience when negotiating got you a lower price, then employ the same tactics.

While heuristics may be helpful when facing smaller issues, major decisions shouldn't necessarily be made using a shortcut approach. Heuristics also don't guarantee an effective solution, such as when trying to drive around a traffic jam only to find yourself on an equally crowded route.

Trial and Error

A trial-and-error approach to problem-solving involves trying a number of potential solutions to a particular issue, then ruling out those that do not work. If you're not sure whether to buy a shirt in blue or green, for instance, you may try on each before deciding which one to purchase.

This can be a good strategy to use if you have a limited number of solutions available. But if there are many different choices available, narrowing down the possible options using another problem-solving technique can be helpful before attempting trial and error.

In some cases, the solution to a problem can appear as a sudden insight. You are facing an issue in a relationship or your career when, out of nowhere, the solution appears in your mind and you know exactly what to do.

Insight can occur when the problem in front of you is similar to an issue that you've dealt with in the past. Although, you may not recognize what is occurring since the underlying mental processes that lead to insight often happen outside of conscious awareness .

Research indicates that insight is most likely to occur during times when you are alone—such as when going on a walk by yourself, when you're in the shower, or when lying in bed after waking up.

How to Apply Problem-Solving Strategies in Real Life

If you're facing a problem, you can implement one or more of these strategies to find a potential solution. Here's how to use them in real life:

  • Create a flow chart . If you have time, you can take advantage of the algorithm approach to problem-solving by sitting down and making a flow chart of each potential solution, its consequences, and what happens next.
  • Recall your past experiences . When a problem needs to be solved fairly quickly, heuristics may be a better approach. Think back to when you faced a similar issue, then use your knowledge and experience to choose the best option possible.
  • Start trying potential solutions . If your options are limited, start trying them one by one to see which solution is best for achieving your desired goal. If a particular solution doesn't work, move on to the next.
  • Take some time alone . Since insight is often achieved when you're alone, carve out time to be by yourself for a while. The answer to your problem may come to you, seemingly out of the blue, if you spend some time away from others.

Obstacles to Problem-Solving

Problem-solving is not a flawless process as there are a number of obstacles that can interfere with our ability to solve a problem quickly and efficiently. These obstacles include:

  • Assumptions: When dealing with a problem, people can make assumptions about the constraints and obstacles that prevent certain solutions. Thus, they may not even try some potential options.
  • Functional fixedness : This term refers to the tendency to view problems only in their customary manner. Functional fixedness prevents people from fully seeing all of the different options that might be available to find a solution.
  • Irrelevant or misleading information: When trying to solve a problem, it's important to distinguish between information that is relevant to the issue and irrelevant data that can lead to faulty solutions. The more complex the problem, the easier it is to focus on misleading or irrelevant information.
  • Mental set: A mental set is a tendency to only use solutions that have worked in the past rather than looking for alternative ideas. A mental set can work as a heuristic, making it a useful problem-solving tool. However, mental sets can also lead to inflexibility, making it more difficult to find effective solutions.

How to Improve Your Problem-Solving Skills

In the end, if your goal is to become a better problem-solver, it's helpful to remember that this is a process. Thus, if you want to improve your problem-solving skills, following these steps can help lead you to your solution:

  • Recognize that a problem exists . If you are facing a problem, there are generally signs. For instance, if you have a mental illness , you may experience excessive fear or sadness, mood changes, and changes in sleeping or eating habits. Recognizing these signs can help you realize that an issue exists.
  • Decide to solve the problem . Make a conscious decision to solve the issue at hand. Commit to yourself that you will go through the steps necessary to find a solution.
  • Seek to fully understand the issue . Analyze the problem you face, looking at it from all sides. If your problem is relationship-related, for instance, ask yourself how the other person may be interpreting the issue. You might also consider how your actions might be contributing to the situation.
  • Research potential options . Using the problem-solving strategies mentioned, research potential solutions. Make a list of options, then consider each one individually. What are some pros and cons of taking the available routes? What would you need to do to make them happen?
  • Take action . Select the best solution possible and take action. Action is one of the steps required for change . So, go through the motions needed to resolve the issue.
  • Try another option, if needed . If the solution you chose didn't work, don't give up. Either go through the problem-solving process again or simply try another option.

You can find a way to solve your problems as long as you keep working toward this goal—even if the best solution is simply to let go because no other good solution exists.

Sarathy V. Real world problem-solving .  Front Hum Neurosci . 2018;12:261. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00261

Dunbar K. Problem solving . A Companion to Cognitive Science . 2017. doi:10.1002/9781405164535.ch20

Stewart SL, Celebre A, Hirdes JP, Poss JW. Risk of suicide and self-harm in kids: The development of an algorithm to identify high-risk individuals within the children's mental health system . Child Psychiat Human Develop . 2020;51:913-924. doi:10.1007/s10578-020-00968-9

Rosenbusch H, Soldner F, Evans AM, Zeelenberg M. Supervised machine learning methods in psychology: A practical introduction with annotated R code . Soc Personal Psychol Compass . 2021;15(2):e12579. doi:10.1111/spc3.12579

Mishra S. Decision-making under risk: Integrating perspectives from biology, economics, and psychology . Personal Soc Psychol Rev . 2014;18(3):280-307. doi:10.1177/1088868314530517

Csikszentmihalyi M, Sawyer K. Creative insight: The social dimension of a solitary moment . In: The Systems Model of Creativity . 2015:73-98. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9085-7_7

Chrysikou EG, Motyka K, Nigro C, Yang SI, Thompson-Schill SL. Functional fixedness in creative thinking tasks depends on stimulus modality .  Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts . 2016;10(4):425‐435. doi:10.1037/aca0000050

Huang F, Tang S, Hu Z. Unconditional perseveration of the short-term mental set in chunk decomposition .  Front Psychol . 2018;9:2568. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02568

National Alliance on Mental Illness. Warning signs and symptoms .

Mayer RE. Thinking, problem solving, cognition, 2nd ed .

Schooler JW, Ohlsson S, Brooks K. Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. J Experiment Psychol: General . 1993;122:166-183. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.2.166

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Logo

12 Apr What Affects Problem Solving

Many factors affect the problem solving process and hence it can become complicated and drawn out when they are unaccounted for. Acknowledging the factors that affect the process and taking them into account when forming a solution gives teams the best chance of solving the problem effectively. Below we have outlined the key factors affecting the problem solving process.

Understanding the problem 

The most important factor in solving a problem is to first fully understand it. This includes understanding the bigger picture it sits within, the factors and stakeholders involved, the causes of the problem and any potential solutions. Effective solutions are unlikely to be discovered if the exact problem is not fully understood.

Personality types/Temperament 

McCauley (1987) was one of the first authors to link personalities to problem solving skills. Attributes like patience, communication, team skills and cognitive skills can all affect an individual’s likelihood of solving a problem. Different individuals will take different approaches to solving problems and experience varying degrees of success. For this reason, as a manager, it is important to select team members for a project whose skills align with the problem at hand.

Skills/Competencies

Individual’s skills will also affect the problem solving process. For example, a straight-forward technical issue may appear very complicated to an individual from a non-technical background. Skill levels are most commonly determined by experience and training and for this reason it is important to expose newer team members to a wide variety of problems, as well as providing training.

Resources available

Although many individuals believe they have the capabilities to solve a certain problem, the resources available to them can often slow-down the process. These resources may be in the form of technology, human capital or finance. For example, a team may come up with a solution for an inefficient transport system by suggesting new vehicles are purchased. Despite the solution solving the problem entirely, it may not fit within the budget. This is why only realistic solutions should be pursued and resources should not be wasted on other projects.

External factors should also always be taken into account when solving a problem, as factors that may not seem to directly affect the problem can often play a part. Examples include competitor actions, fluctuations in the economy, government restrictions and environmental issues.

Carskadon, Thomas G, Nancy G McCarley, and Mary H McCaulley. (1987). Compendium of Research Involving the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator . Gainesville, Fl.: Center for Applications of Psychological Type, 1987. Print.

Technical Questions and Answers

  • Trending Categories

Data Structure

  • Selected Reading
  • UPSC IAS Exams Notes
  • Developer's Best Practices
  • Questions and Answers
  • Effective Resume Writing
  • HR Interview Questions
  • Computer Glossary

Facilitating and Hindering Factors in Problem Solving

When a person is in an examination hall trying to solve a problem, and suddenly the teacher stands above that person, the student feels nervous and may solve the problem incorrectly. Common scenario? Another example could be when a person lives where the temperature is too high. Could this affect problem-solving? If certain factors could reduce the capacity to solve a problem, then can certain factors also heighten the skills to solve problems?

Problem Solving

Problem-solving refers to the cognitive processes involved in finding a solution to a problem. Nevertheless, it is well known that environmental factors affect our cognitive processes. Since problem solving also involves cognition, it is also affected by many factors. Not every solution needs to be effective. It may happen that the first solution that comes to mind is not the most effective, and the more the solution is brainstormed and discussed, the better solution one can come up with.

  • Barriers to Problem Solving

The following factors obstruct problem-solving

Mental set − Mental set refers to our tendency to solve certain problems the same way we used to. These are fixed ways of solving a problem using old methods; there is no other way of thinking. For example, a student has three papers lined up and has to study for them all. The most common solution would be to study the first paper that is coming, but it may happen that this might not be effective. Another solution can be to study difficult, moderate, and then easy subjects or to study the easy ones first and then move on to the hard ones.

Functional fixedness − in this sense, it is the tendency to think of the most common use of any object. For example, everyone has seen puzzles like "Use this amount of matchsticks to make this or that object. However, most people cannot solve these puzzles, as they are fixed on the number or the most common use of the object. This involves abstract thinking to solve a problem, but functional fixedness could restrain problem-solving skills.

Irrelevant information − At all times, there are hundreds of stimuli around us in every form, be it audio, visual, or tactile, but we are exposed to various stimuli. Much information could be irrelevant to our problem-solving, creating dissatisfaction. For example, while someone is telling a riddle and we need to solve it, if one has observed much irrelevant information being given out and the answer is pretty obvious, but due to that irrelevant information, we pay attention to the information and thus miss out on the answer.

Insight − When we face unfamiliar situations, the old solutions do not help. The same perspective does not help in finding the solution to the problem. To get more insight into the problem, a different perspective is needed. However, as stated above, it is sometimes difficult for us to see things from a different perspective.

Resources available − Sometimes, even when we have found the solution, applying it to the problem is what matters. One thing to be noticed when we find a solution is its effectiveness. A person may not have all the needed resources, which can hinder solving a problem.

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Facilitating Factors

Though many factors hinder problem-solving, on the other hand, there are facilitating factors as well:

Motivation − motivation is positively correlated with problem-solving skills. This means that the higher the motivation, the more efficient problem-solving is. For example, a person has to finish the presentation by a deadline, and this high motivation will encourage the person to look for solutions on how he can complete the presentation in the stipulated time. Motivation fuels the person internally and encourages him or her to achieve the goal more efficiently.

Self-efficacy − self-efficacy refers to the belief in one's capabilities. When a person is self-efficient, then the person knows that he will find the solution. This self-belief pushes the person to brainstorm and think about various strategies they could adopt to solve the problem.

Perception − Perception is highly correlated with problem-solving abilities. This indicates that we can find a solution when we see a problem as something that could be solved or a challenge. Nevertheless, on the other hand, if the person sees the problem as a threat or harm, the approach to that problem would be very different.

Teamwork skills − Most solutions are found when they are discussed and debated. When a person works in a team, s/he is exposed to a +variety of ideas and attitudes. This helps in finding an effective solution or modifying a solution that would be adaptive.

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Measures to bring about Improvements in Problem Solving Skills

The following are the measures that are implemented to improve problem-solving abilities

Acquire Technical Knowledge in Your Field − It is well acknowledged that in order to carry out one's jobs and activities, people rely heavily on technology. Individuals must be knowledgeable about how to use technology.

Look for chances to solve problems − There are several chances accessible for delivering answers to challenges. However, these possibilities must be pursued. Individuals must raise awareness and expand their grasp of the opportunities. In certain circumstances, managers and teachers raise individuals' awareness of available options; in other circumstances, they produce their information.

Implement Practice and Role Play Techniques − Practice and role play techniques are thought to help improve problem-solving skills. Man becomes flawless via practice, and individuals who engage in frequent practice can propose answers to various difficulties. Analytical, technical, logical, methodical, mechanical, procedural, and so on are examples.

Examine how other people solve problems − Observing is a vital method of learning and thoroughly comprehending concepts and elements. It applies to all regions and fields, and this strategy significantly improves problem-solving abilities.

Problem-solving skills are cognitive processes that aim to solve a problem. As we are exposed to various situations with novel problems every second day, many factors prevent a person from finding a solution. The factors that act as hindrances are mental set, functional fixedness, irrelevant information, insight, and resources available. Nevertheless, on the other hand, the facilitating factors are motivation, perception, self-efficacy, and teamwork skills. Even though there are some common factors, due to individual differences, factors might vary in intensity or their presence or absence. Problem-solving skills are very much needed in every stage of life, and these factors can be controlled to facilitate problem-solving skills.

Utkarsh Shukla

  • Related Articles
  • Attention and Problem Solving
  • Reasoning and Problem Solving
  • Problem Solving: Meaning, Theory, and Strategies
  • Problem Solving - Steps, Techniques, & Best Practices
  • Problem-solving on Boolean Model and Vector Space Model
  • Tips for Effective Problem-solving in Quality Management
  • How Can Leaders Improve Problem-Solving Abilities?
  • Explain The Scientific Method Used By A Scientist In Solving Problem?
  • Solving Cryptarithmetic Puzzles
  • Sudoku Solving algorithms
  • Signals and Systems – Solving Differential Equations with Laplace Transform
  • Water and Jug Problem in C++
  • Snake and Ladder Problem
  • Nuts and Bolt Problem

Kickstart Your Career

Get certified by completing the course

Listen-Hard

Identifying Barriers to Problem-Solving in Psychology

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Problem-solving is a key aspect of psychology, essential for understanding and overcoming challenges in our daily lives. There are common barriers that can hinder our ability to effectively solve problems. From mental blocks to confirmation bias, these obstacles can impede our progress.

In this article, we will explore the various barriers to problem-solving in psychology, as well as strategies to overcome them. By addressing these challenges head-on, we can unlock the benefits of improved problem-solving skills and mental agility.

  • Identifying and overcoming barriers to problem-solving in psychology can lead to more effective and efficient solutions.
  • Some common barriers include mental blocks, confirmation bias, and functional fixedness, which can all limit critical thinking and creativity.
  • Mindfulness techniques, seeking different perspectives, and collaborating with others can help overcome these barriers and lead to more successful problem-solving.
  • 1 What Is Problem-Solving in Psychology?
  • 2 Why Is Problem-Solving Important in Psychology?
  • 3.1 Mental Blocks
  • 3.2 Confirmation Bias
  • 3.3 Functional Fixedness
  • 3.4 Lack of Creativity
  • 3.5 Emotional Barriers
  • 3.6 Cultural Influences
  • 4.1 Divergent Thinking
  • 4.2 Mindfulness Techniques
  • 4.3 Seeking Different Perspectives
  • 4.4 Challenging Assumptions
  • 4.5 Collaborating with Others
  • 5 What Are the Benefits of Overcoming These Barriers?
  • 6 Frequently Asked Questions

What Is Problem-Solving in Psychology?

Problem-solving in psychology refers to the cognitive processes through which individuals identify and overcome obstacles or challenges to reach a desired goal, drawing on various mental processes and strategies.

In the realm of cognitive psychology, problem-solving is a key area of study that delves into how people use algorithms and heuristics to tackle complex issues. Algorithms are systematic step-by-step procedures that guarantee a solution, whereas heuristics are mental shortcuts or rules of thumb that provide efficient solutions, albeit without certainty. Understanding these mental processes is crucial in exploring how individuals approach different types of problems and make decisions based on their problem-solving strategies.

Why Is Problem-Solving Important in Psychology?

Problem-solving holds significant importance in psychology as it facilitates the discovery of new insights, enhances understanding of complex issues, and fosters effective actions based on informed decisions.

Assumptions play a crucial role in problem-solving processes, influencing how individuals perceive and approach challenges. By challenging these assumptions, individuals can break through mental barriers and explore creative solutions.

Functional fixedness, a cognitive bias where individuals restrict the use of objects to their traditional functions, can hinder problem-solving. Overcoming functional fixedness involves reevaluating the purpose of objects, leading to innovative problem-solving strategies.

Through problem-solving, psychologists uncover underlying patterns in behavior, delve into subconscious motivations, and offer practical interventions to improve mental well-being.

What Are the Common Barriers to Problem-Solving in Psychology?

In psychology, common barriers to problem-solving include mental blocks , confirmation bias , functional fixedness, lack of creativity, emotional barriers, and cultural influences that hinder the application of knowledge and resources to overcome challenges.

Mental blocks refer to the difficulty in generating new ideas or solutions due to preconceived notions or past experiences. Confirmation bias, on the other hand, is the tendency to search for, interpret, or prioritize information that confirms existing beliefs or hypotheses, while disregarding opposing evidence.

Functional fixedness limits problem-solving by constraining individuals to view objects or concepts in their traditional uses, inhibiting creative approaches. Lack of creativity impedes the ability to think outside the box and consider unconventional solutions.

Emotional barriers such as fear, stress, or anxiety can halt progress by clouding judgment and hindering clear decision-making. Cultural influences may introduce unique perspectives or expectations that clash with effective problem-solving strategies, complicating the resolution process.

Mental Blocks

Mental blocks in problem-solving occur when individuals struggle to consider all relevant information, fall into a fixed mental set, or become fixated on irrelevant details, hindering progress and creative solutions.

For instance, irrelevant information can lead to mental blocks by distracting individuals from focusing on the key elements required to solve a problem effectively. This could involve getting caught up in minor details that have no real impact on the overall solution. A fixed mental set, formed by previous experiences or patterns, can limit one’s ability to approach a problem from new perspectives, restricting innovative thinking.

Confirmation Bias

Confirmation bias, a common barrier in problem-solving, leads individuals to seek information that confirms their existing knowledge or assumptions, potentially overlooking contradictory data and hindering objective analysis.

This cognitive bias affects decision-making and problem-solving processes by creating a tendency to favor information that aligns with one’s beliefs, rather than considering all perspectives.

  • One effective method to mitigate confirmation bias is by actively challenging assumptions through critical thinking.
  • By questioning the validity of existing beliefs and seeking out diverse viewpoints, individuals can counteract the tendency to only consider information that confirms their preconceptions.
  • Another strategy is to promote a culture of open-mindedness and encourage constructive debate within teams to foster a more comprehensive evaluation of data.

Functional Fixedness

Functional fixedness restricts problem-solving by limiting individuals to conventional uses of objects, impeding the discovery of innovative solutions and hindering the application of insightful approaches to challenges.

For instance, when faced with a task that requires a candle to be mounted on a wall to provide lighting, someone bound by functional fixedness may struggle to see the potential solution of using the candle wax as an adhesive instead of solely perceiving the candle’s purpose as a light source.

This mental rigidity often leads individuals to overlook unconventional or creative methods, which can stifle their ability to find effective problem-solving strategies.

To combat this cognitive limitation, fostering divergent thinking, encouraging experimentation, and promoting flexibility in approaching tasks can help individuals break free from functional fixedness and unlock their creativity.

Lack of Creativity

A lack of creativity poses a significant barrier to problem-solving, limiting the potential for improvement and hindering flexible thinking required to generate novel solutions and address complex challenges.

When individuals are unable to think outside the box and explore unconventional approaches, they may find themselves stuck in repetitive patterns without breakthroughs.

Flexibility is key to overcoming this hurdle, allowing individuals to adapt their perspectives, pivot when necessary, and consider multiple viewpoints to arrive at innovative solutions.

Encouraging a culture that embraces experimentation, values diverse ideas, and fosters an environment of continuous learning can fuel creativity and push problem-solving capabilities to new heights.

Emotional Barriers

Emotional barriers, such as fear of failure, can impede problem-solving by creating anxiety, reducing risk-taking behavior, and hindering effective collaboration with others, limiting the exploration of innovative solutions.

When individuals are held back by the fear of failure, it often stems from a deep-seated worry about making mistakes or being judged negatively. This fear can lead to hesitation in decision-making processes and reluctance to explore unconventional approaches, ultimately hindering the ability to discover creative solutions. To overcome this obstacle, it is essential to cultivate a positive emotional environment that fosters trust, resilience, and open communication among team members. Encouraging a mindset that embraces failure as a stepping stone to success can enable individuals to take risks, learn from setbacks, and collaborate effectively to overcome challenges.

Cultural Influences

Cultural influences can act as barriers to problem-solving by imposing rigid norms, limiting flexibility in thinking, and hindering effective communication and collaboration among diverse individuals with varying perspectives.

When individuals from different cultural backgrounds come together to solve problems, the ingrained values and beliefs they hold can shape their approaches and methods.

For example, in some cultures, decisiveness and quick decision-making are highly valued, while in others, a consensus-building process is preferred.

Understanding and recognizing these differences is crucial for navigating through the cultural barriers that might arise during collaborative problem-solving.

How Can These Barriers Be Overcome?

These barriers to problem-solving in psychology can be overcome through various strategies such as divergent thinking, mindfulness techniques, seeking different perspectives, challenging assumptions, and collaborating with others to leverage diverse insights and foster critical thinking.

Engaging in divergent thinking , which involves generating multiple solutions or viewpoints for a single issue, can help break away from conventional problem-solving methods. By encouraging a free flow of ideas without immediate judgment, individuals can explore innovative paths that may lead to breakthrough solutions. Actively seeking diverse perspectives from individuals with varied backgrounds, experiences, and expertise can offer fresh insights that challenge existing assumptions and broaden the problem-solving scope. This diversity of viewpoints can spark creativity and unconventional approaches that enhance problem-solving outcomes.

Divergent Thinking

Divergent thinking enhances problem-solving by encouraging creative exploration of multiple solutions, breaking habitual thought patterns, and fostering flexibility in generating innovative ideas to address challenges.

When individuals engage in divergent thinking, they open up their minds to various possibilities and perspectives. Instead of being constrained by conventional norms, a person might ideate freely without limitations. This leads to out-of-the-box solutions that can revolutionize how problems are approached. Divergent thinking sparks creativity by allowing unconventional ideas to surface and flourish.

For example, imagine a team tasked with redesigning a city park. Instead of sticking to traditional layouts, they might brainstorm wild concepts like turning the park into a futuristic playground, a pop-up art gallery space, or a wildlife sanctuary. Such diverse ideas stem from divergent thinking and push boundaries beyond the ordinary.

Mindfulness Techniques

Mindfulness techniques can aid problem-solving by promoting present-moment awareness, reducing cognitive biases, and fostering a habit of continuous learning that enhances adaptability and open-mindedness in addressing challenges.

Engaging in regular mindfulness practices encourages individuals to stay grounded in the current moment, allowing them to detach from preconceived notions and biases that could cloud judgment. By cultivating a non-judgmental attitude towards thoughts and emotions, people develop the capacity to observe situations from a neutral perspective, facilitating clearer decision-making processes. Mindfulness techniques facilitate the development of a growth mindset, where one acknowledges mistakes as opportunities for learning and improvement rather than failures.

Seeking Different Perspectives

Seeking different perspectives in problem-solving involves tapping into diverse resources, engaging in effective communication, and considering alternative viewpoints to broaden understanding and identify innovative solutions to complex issues.

Collaboration among individuals with various backgrounds and experiences can offer fresh insights and approaches to tackling challenges. By fostering an environment where all voices are valued and heard, teams can leverage the collective wisdom and creativity present in diverse perspectives. For example, in the tech industry, companies like Google encourage cross-functional teams to work together, harnessing diverse skill sets to develop groundbreaking technologies.

To incorporate diverse viewpoints, one can implement brainstorming sessions that involve individuals from different departments or disciplines to encourage out-of-the-box thinking. Another effective method is to conduct surveys or focus groups to gather input from a wide range of stakeholders and ensure inclusivity in decision-making processes.

Challenging Assumptions

Challenging assumptions is a key strategy in problem-solving, as it prompts individuals to critically evaluate preconceived notions, gain new insights, and expand their knowledge base to approach challenges from fresh perspectives.

By questioning established beliefs or ways of thinking, individuals open the door to innovative solutions and original perspectives. Stepping outside the boundaries of conventional wisdom enables problem solvers to see beyond limitations and explore uncharted territories. This process not only fosters creativity but also encourages a culture of continuous improvement where learning thrives. Daring to challenge assumptions can unveil hidden opportunities and untapped potential in problem-solving scenarios, leading to breakthroughs and advancements that were previously overlooked.

  • One effective technique to challenge assumptions is through brainstorming sessions that encourage participants to voice unconventional ideas without judgment.
  • Additionally, adopting a beginner’s mindset can help in questioning assumptions, as newcomers often bring a fresh perspective unburdened by past biases.

Collaborating with Others

Collaborating with others in problem-solving fosters flexibility, encourages open communication, and leverages collective intelligence to navigate complex challenges, drawing on diverse perspectives and expertise to generate innovative solutions.

Effective collaboration enables individuals to combine strengths and talents, pooling resources to tackle problems that may seem insurmountable when approached individually. By working together, team members can break down barriers and silos that often hinder progress, leading to more efficient problem-solving processes and better outcomes.

Collaboration also promotes a sense of shared purpose and increases overall engagement, as team members feel valued and enableed to contribute their unique perspectives. To foster successful collaboration, it is crucial to establish clear goals, roles, and communication channels, ensuring that everyone is aligned towards a common objective.

What Are the Benefits of Overcoming These Barriers?

Overcoming the barriers to problem-solving in psychology leads to significant benefits such as improved critical thinking skills, enhanced knowledge acquisition, and the ability to address complex issues with greater creativity and adaptability.

By mastering the art of problem-solving, individuals in the field of psychology can also cultivate resilience and perseverance, two essential traits that contribute to personal growth and success.

When confronting and overcoming cognitive obstacles, individuals develop a deeper understanding of their own cognitive processes and behavioral patterns, enabling them to make informed decisions and overcome challenges more effectively.

Continuous learning and adaptability play a pivotal role in problem-solving, allowing psychologists to stay updated with the latest research, techniques, and methodologies that enhance their problem-solving capabilities.

Frequently Asked Questions

Similar posts.

Exploring the Concept of Social Scripts in Psychology

Exploring the Concept of Social Scripts in Psychology

The article was last updated by Marcus Wong on February 5, 2024. Have you ever found yourself acting in a certain way in social situations…

Uncovering the Meaning of Blind Spots in Psychology

Uncovering the Meaning of Blind Spots in Psychology

The article was last updated by Dr. Emily Tan on February 8, 2024. Ever find yourself making decisions that seem irrational in hindsight? Or perhaps…

Unveiling Memory Stages: Psychology Experts and the Second Stage

Unveiling Memory Stages: Psychology Experts and the Second Stage

The article was last updated by Rachel Liu on February 5, 2024. Have you ever wondered how your memory works and the different stages it…

Exploring Stress Tests in Evolutionary Psychology

Exploring Stress Tests in Evolutionary Psychology

The article was last updated by Ethan Clarke on February 8, 2024. Have you ever wondered why humans behave the way they do? Evolutionary psychology…

Informed Consent in Psychology: Understanding its Gestures and Limitations

Informed Consent in Psychology: Understanding its Gestures and Limitations

The article was last updated by Julian Torres on February 5, 2024. In psychology, informed consent plays a crucial role in ensuring ethical practices and…

Exploring Dr. Spellman’s Contributions to Psychology: What Did He Study?

Exploring Dr. Spellman’s Contributions to Psychology: What Did He Study?

The article was last updated by Lena Nguyen on February 9, 2024. Have you ever wondered who Dr. Spellman is and what contributions he made…

  • Open access
  • Published: 25 May 2023

Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective learning: results from partial least square structural equation modeling and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis

  • Ying Wang 1   na1 ,
  • Ze-Ling Xu 1   na1 ,
  • Jia-Yao Lou 1   na1 &
  • Ke-Da Chen 1  

BMC Medical Education volume  23 , Article number:  382 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

5935 Accesses

2 Citations

Metrics details

The Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development emphasizes the importance of complex problem-solving (CPS) skills in the 21st century. CPS skills have been linked to academic performance, career development, and job competency training. Reflective learning, which includes journal writing, peer reflection, selfreflection, and group discussion, has been explored to improve critical thinking and problem-solving abilities. The development of various thinking modes and abilities, such as algorithmic thinking, creativity, and empathic concern, all affect problem-solving skills. However, there is a lack of an overall theory to relate variables to each other, which means that different theories need to be integrated to focus on how CPS skills can be effectively trained and improved.

Data from 136 medical students were analyzed using partial least square structural equation modeling (PLSSEM) and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA). A hypothesized model examining the associations between the CPS skills and influence factors was constructed.

The evaluation of the structural model showed that some variables had significant influences on CPS skills, while others did not. After deleting the insignificant pathways, a structural model was built, which showed that mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking were observed, while personal distress only had a direct effect on CPS skills. The results of necessity showed that only cooperativity and creativity are necessary conditions for critical thinking. The fsQCA analysis provided clues for each different pathway to the result, with all consistency values being higher than 0.8, and most coverage values being between 0.240 and 0.839. The fsQCA confirmed the validity of the model and provided configurations that enhanced the CPS skills.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence that reflective learning based on multi-dimensional empathy theory and 21 stcentury skills theory can improve CPS skills in medical students. These results have practical implications for learning and suggest that educators should consider incorporating reflective learning strategies that focus on empathy and 21 stcentury skills to enhance CPS skills in their curricula.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

When putting forward the theoretical framework of skills and competencies in the 21st century, the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development takes complex problem-solving (CPS) skills as an important component and brings them into the evaluation system of the Program for International Student Assessment [ 1 ]. Previous research results have proved that there is a significant positive correlation between CPS skills and academic performance [ 2 ], that is, the stronger the problem-solving skill, the better the academic performance. Similarly, it is also considered to have a great influence on career selection [ 3 ], career development [ 4 ], and job competency training [ 5 ]. Therefore, the improvement of the above-mentioned comprehensive qualities, such as learning ability and post competence, and the cultivation of CPS skills, has been emphasized by a variety of teaching strategies, such as problem-based learning (PBL) [ 6 ], context-based learning (CBL) [ 7 ], situational simulation [ 8 ], and reflective learning [ 9 ].

As an important process of metacognition, reflective learning is closely related to CPS skills. Gadbury-Amyot et al. claimed that the use of reflection and writing as educational strategies to promote critical thinking and problem-solving is one of the best ways for students to express their thought processes [ 10 ]. Exploration to improve CPS skills based on reflective learning and training can be seen in medicine, computer science, mathematics, and other industries. According to Bernack, establishing problem-solving training courses could feasibly enhance the abilities of pre-service teachers [ 11 ]. Kellogg suggested that reflection and writing, as educational strategies to promote critical thinking and problem-solving skill, is one of the best ways to improve students’ expression ability and logical thinking [ 12 ]. “Reflective learning” is a common way of exploring problems and solutions in the deliberative environment, a process of learning through experience, and is a necessary learning tool in professional education [ 13 ]. Reflective learning includes journal writing, peer reflection, self-reflection, and group discussion under the guidance of teachers [ 14 ]. Illeris suggested that the result of reflective learning spans cognitive, psychodynamic, and social-societal dimensions [ 15 ]. Through its influence on students’ behavior, thoughts, and emotions, it realizes the training and improvement of students’ various abilities. It has gradually developed into a more efficient and autonomous learning model and has become an indispensable educational and learning tool for many professionals [ 16 ]. Many experts suggest that the implementation of reflective learning can improve students’ critical thinking [ 17 ], insight [ 18 ], empathic concern [ 19 ], computational thinking [ 20 ], and other skills, and this improvement of a variety of thinking modes and abilities will eventually lead to improvement of their CPS skills [ 21 ]. Our research on the factors affecting the CPS skills is based on reflective learning.

The purpose of human problem solving is to promote the understanding of human thinking through a detailed investigation of the way people solve difficult problems, such as logic or chess. Unlike computer simulations, human problem solving is influenced by psychological factors that cannot be ignored [ 22 ]. Therefore, problem solving is dynamic and needs to consider the influence of speculation, social background, and culture, while CPS skills emphasize the process of successful interaction between the problem solver and the dynamic task environment [ 23 ]. CPS skills are collections of self-regulating psychological processes necessary in the face of complex and dynamic non-routine situations across different domains [ 24 ], and comprises a combination of skills, abilities, motivation, and other psychological structures [ 25 , 26 ]. The factors that impact CPS skills are complex and include cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Research shows that the development of a variety of thinking modes and abilities, such as algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, creativity, critical thinking, personal distress, fantasy, perspective-taking, and empathic concern, all affect the problem-solving skill in varying degrees [ 27 , 28 ]. Among them, empathetic concern and critical thinking have been proven to affect problem-solving skill by many studies. After comprehensively exploring the emerging research on the impact of the factors on the CPS skills, we found that previous studies mainly focused on a single causality in the improvement of problem-solving skill, while there is a lack of overall theory to relate variables to each other, which means that we need to integrate different theories to advance existing research and focuson how CPS skills can be effectively trained and improved.

Theoretical background

The literature analysis of CPS skills reveals the current research status. Based on the relevant theories of skills needed in the 21st century [ 1 ], individuals use analytical, reasoning, and cooperative skills to identify and solve problems consistent with their areas of interest [ 29 ]. Kocak proposes that problem-solving skills are shaped by algorithmic thinking, creativity, cooperativity, critical thinking, digital literacy, and effective communication, and develops a model with critical thinking as a mediating factor [ 21 ]. Developing solutions for complex problems is a complicated process, and individuals require critical thinking skills [ 21 , 30 ] to do so. Critical thinking often occurs at the same time as CPS skills and is one of the core objectives of general education in all subjects of higher education [ 29 ]. Critical thinking, closely related to reflective learning [ 17 ], which has been emphasized in many studies, especially in the implementation of learning strategies including reflective learning. In problem-based learning and case-based learning, instructors encourage learners to use critical reflection to engage with subject matter and to develop their own practice in closing any knowledge gaps that may exist [ 31 ]. Additionally, digital literacy involves the ability to assess the accuracy and value of online resources [ 32 ]. In this study, reflective learning was the primary learning strategy [ 33 ]; therefore, digital literacy skills were not observed in detail. Drawing on the above analysis, we developed a theoretical model that identifies algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity as antecedents, and critical thinking as an intermediary variable that influences CPS skills.

Another major area related to affecting CPS skills is empathic concern. The research suggests that students with a higher level of cognitive empathy show more positive attitudes and deal with problems more effectively [ 34 ]. In essence, empathetic concern fosters values, beliefs, attitudes, and assumptions, and affect the CPS skills from the perspective of execution [ 35 , 36 ]. Some studies suggest that reflective learning improves empathy [ 37 ]. Based on Davis’s Interpersonal Reactivity Index [ 38 ], empathy was divided into four dimensions mentioned: Empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress. Nevertheless, some scholars disagree that personal distress belongs to the category of empathy. Personal distress is defined as an over-arousal caused by the lack of boundaries between oneself and others [ 39 , 40 ]. Some studies show that personal distress leads to egoism and overwhelms altruistic activities mediated by empathetic concern [ 41 ]. And there is a statistically significant correlation between personal distress and empathetic concern [ 42 ]. Therefore, we still adhere to the view that the two cannot be regarded as mutually exclusive emotions, bringing personal distress into the scope of our research and exploring its role in CPS skills. Empathetic concern has been proved to associate with prosocial behavior [ 43 ]. In the relationship between empathic concern and prosocial concern, empathic concern elicits an approach orientation toward the target [ 44 ] and is used as a mediator variable in some models. For example, some studies consider empathic concern and personal distress are both mediators of perspective-taking to helping behavior [ 45 ]. Based on the above analysis, we built our theoretical model and assume that personal distress, perspective-taking, and fantasy as antecedents and empathetic concern as intermediary variables that affect the CPS skills.

Above all, the empathic concern and the critical thinking are two remarkable characteristics of the CPS skills, which can play a common role in the CPS skills [ 46 ], however, there is a lack of overall theory to connect them, which means that different theories need to be integrated to promote research. The comprehensive study of the combination of the two aspects can better understand how to improve CPS skills, which cannot be provided by any theory alone. Moreover, the results on the factors affecting the CPS skills also show some inconsistencies. For example, Batson believes that personal distress in empathy inhibits the development of problem-solving skills [ 41 ], whereas Mora disagrees [ 47 ]. A possible reasonable explanation for these contradictory results is that the previous studies on the factors influencing problem-solving skill mainly adopted traditional symmetric methods (such as regression and SEM), which did not fully capture the complexity of the factors that influence problem-solving skills, and the factors affecting the CPS skills are often based on multiple causalities rather than a single causal relationship. Simply evaluating symmetric relationships might lead to divergent results, thus masking the complexity of the problem-solving skill. Considering the complex nature of CPS skills under the condition of reflective learning, it is necessary to check the symmetric and asymmetric relationships between structures to fully understand the strategies and methods to improve CPS skills, therefore, PLS-SEM [ 48 ] and fsQCA [ 49 ] were used in our study comprehensively.

Research model and hypothesis development

Designing the pls-sem research model.

Critical thinking in the field of cognition and empathic concern in the field of emotion are representatives of two different thinking modes affecting the CPS skills. PLS-SEM assumes that fantasy, perspective-taking, personal distress, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity have a direct impact on the CPS skills. Empathetic concern and critical thinking play an intermediary role between these relationships and the CPS skills (Fig.  1 A).

figure 1

Partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) conceptual model and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA) conceptual model: ( A ) The PLS-SEM conceptual model. ( B ) The fsQCA conceptual model

Personal distress and CPS skills

The definition of personal distress in this study pertains to the discomfort and anxiety that respondents experience when observing negative experiences of others, including fear, apprehension, and discomfort. Personal distress is an aspect of emotional empathy [ 38 ]. Some studies show that personal distress and empathy are complex and dynamic emotional experiences [ 50 ]. Personal distress, as an indicator of self-other differentiation and emotional regulation, is a kind of negative emotion. Excessive personal distress can lead to emotional regulation and interpersonal difficulties [ 51 ]. These studies advocate reducing personal distress to relieve stress [ 52 ]. The healthcare sector prioritizes a patient-centered healthcare model, which mandates that we respond to patients’ emotional distress with this principle in mind. However, in practice, health professionals tend to regard emotional health problems as “routine”; therefore, it is necessary to put patients’ emotional and identity issues in the dominant position of the marginal biomedical model used by health professionals [ 53 ]. However, empathic pain is crucial to Hoffman’s moral development framework. He believed that pain can cause significant effects that might lead to action [ 54 ]. Moitra’s research also supports the positive effect of personal distress on problem-solving skill [ 47 ]. Reflection encourages individuals to confront their own embarrassing and uncomfortable past experiences, learn from their errors, and enhance their CPS skills [ 55 ].

Fantasy and CPS skills

Fantasy acts on all aspects of reflective learning. First, to some extent, our brains process information and decisions in an irrational way, and reflection contributes to the cultivation of irrational thinking [ 56 ]. The improvement of subjects’ irrational thinking, including fantasy, can be promoted through reflective learning. Research indicates that individuals with higher fantasy and perspective-taking skills tend to have stronger social understanding [ 57 ]. Second, the development of imagination and fantasy is an important part of cultivating empathic concern [ 58 ]. This is because people understand the world through fantasy, and fantasy gives people hope that the world will become a better place [ 59 ]. For example, Melissa McInnis Brown’s research showed that children who play using fantasies are better at sharing emotions than their peers [ 60 ]. Many studies have proven the role of fantasy in problem-solving. For example, David Weibel pointed out that one can effectively use imagination in an environment, such as in artistic expression or problem-solving [ 61 ]. Fantasy is an imaginative way to find creative solutions that can help people predict the realization of creative structures [ 61 ]. From a sociological point of view, scholars usually regard fantasy as an important factor in cultivating children’s prosocial behaviour [ 57 , 62 ]. Empathic concern requires a person or the whole team to have an overall and largely unconscious “feeling” in terms of emotions, body language, previous experiences, and interpersonal relationships; therefore, this requires significant support from the fantasy system [ 63 ].

Perspective-taking and CPS skills

The effectiveness of group problem solving heavily depends on group member interactions and group composition. For perspective-taking, it provides the possibility for effective communication, which mainly affects the effective presentation of information, effective understanding of that information, conflict resolution, and cooperative interaction [ 64 , 65 ]. In management, perspective-taking has become an important factor in teamwork to solve problems [ 66 ]. During perspective-taking, the problem-solving process can be facilitated by promoting empathic concern, which is evident in the subjects’ cognitive dimension. For example, Falk found that perspective-taking leads to more creative solutions, and team members are more cooperative and facilitate more effective communication [ 64 ]. Bethune and Brown suggested that reflection affects the professional identity of patients by encouraging personal insights and providing different perspectives on patient interaction [ 67 ]. Reflection requires us to think about the past and sum up experiences and lessons from it. Thinking about problems from the standpoint of others can circumvent the limitations of our perspective of looking at problems only through ourselves and can promote the solution of complex problems.

Based on the points discussed above, we propose the following assumptions:

Personal distress is positively related to the CPS skills.

Fantasy is positively related to the CPS skills.

Perspective-taking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Algorithmic thinking and CPS skills

Algorithm thinking draws lessons from the algorithms of computers and artificial intelligence, which enables people to think and deal with things in parallel, process things in data, carry on data and logical reasoning to things, and finally achieve the goal of completing plans and tasks. As one of the core skills in the 21st century, algorithmic thinking abstractly and logically determines the elements used to solve problems through analysis [ 28 ]. One of the major applications of algorithmic thinking is jigsaw puzzle-based learning, which aims to make subjects think about how to build and solve problems, and improve their critical analysis and problem-solving skills [ 68 ]. Hasan Gürbüz leveraged straightforward visual and language templates to help individuals develop models and analyze information about events through games, resulting in improved problem-solving skills [ 69 ]. This mode of thinking, based on logic and steps, is very important for the development of critical thinking and computational thinking [ 28 ]. Many studies have shown that there is a positive correlation between algorithmic thinking and critical thinking [ 70 ]. In reflective learning, algorithmic thinking plays a significant role in computing, as evidenced in this study by recording a short video that necessitates organizing large amounts of data to develop suitable algorithms for analysis [ 71 ].

Creativity and CPS skills

Creativity affects our lives and is vital to the progress of society [ 72 ]. The definition of creativity highlights the integration of novel (original, unexpected) and appropriate (useful, adaptive concerning task constraint) ideas [ 73 ]. Since the 20th century, a large number of scholars in various fields have paid attention to creativity and CPS skills. Creativity is a valuable skill while designing solutions to new challenges that arise in developing societies [ 74 ]. For instance, Garrett noted that creativity plays a crucial role in problem-solving [ 75 ]. In many studies, creativity and critical thinking are interdependent, and creative tasks can improve people’s creativity [ 76 ]. In reflective learning, we utilize divergent thinking that frequently enhances our creativity.

Cooperativity and CPS skills

Many critics believe that cooperativity plays an important role in the cultivation of critical thinking [ 77 ]. Cooperativity receives considerable attention in the learning process due to its association with effective communication. For example, service-learning attaches great importance to cooperation, democratic citizenship, and moral responsibility in the learning process [ 78 ], and preschool educational institutions need to improve the experience through the collaborative exchange, to create favorable conditions for educators to re-examine educational activities, and determine the direction of new relationships through observation [ 79 ]. In reflective learning, subjects become aware of their contradictions and gain valid information, and critically assess peer opinions through active communication, which advances their ideas for program and CPS skills improvement.

Algorithmic thinking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Creativity is positively related to the CPS skills.

Cooperativity is positively related to the CPS skills.

Mediators and CPS skills

This study assumes that empathetic concern and critical thinking act as mediators between the CPS skills and their antecedents.

In Gibbs’s theory, the emotional dimension is a very important aspect of reflective learning [ 80 ]. Madeline Kelly’s research showed that reflection has a positive effect on the improvement of cognitive empathy [ 81 ]; however, there are few studies on the effect of reflective learning on empathy. Cognitive empathy includes fantasy and perspective-taking, while the emotional empathy includes personal distress and empathic concern [ 82 ]. Research shows that the concept of emotional empathy is state empathy, with the focus on altruism [ 83 , 84 ]. Emotional empathy plays an important role in patient-nurse communication [ 85 ]. Failure to deal with or understand emotions will make it difficult for nurses to think rationally and critically about issues that are important to nursing practice [ 86 ]. Therefore, we cannot ignore the influence of empathic concern on the CPS skills in reflective learning. We assumed that the ability of empathic concern can increase altruism and help to improve CPS skills. However, personal distress is usually considered to lead to egoism, which is not conducive to the formation of altruism [ 41 ]. In-depth investigation is necessary to understand its effect on CPS skills. As an important factor in prosocial behavior, the empathic concern serving as a mediator between cognitive behavior and prosocial behavior [ 87 ]. Based on the theories of O’Brien and Gülseven, we constructed a CPS skills model with empathic concern as the mediating variable [ 88 , 89 ].

Effective reflection is characterized by purposeful, focused, and questioning [ 90 ]. In the process of reflection, this mode of thinking requires us to think critically and center on the results. Reflective learning, also known as critical reflection [ 17 ], emphasizes the use of critical thinking. Many critics affirm the results of critical reflection [ 91 , 92 , 93 ]. Parrish and Crookes found that among nursing graduates, reflection helped them to solve problems through thoughtful reasoning and to develop strategies for self-monitoring of their professional competence [ 94 ]. Critical thinking is typically rational thinking, and through combining theory with practice, exploring the similarities and differences between theoretical knowledge and practical experience, and considering a variety of different viewpoints and opinions, the effect of reflective learning can be enhanced. Therefore, speculative reflection is designed to help us identify our shortcomings and think about how to correct and improve them. Critical thinking is widely recognized as an important skill in mediating CPS skills [ 10 ]. Based on the research of Kocak and Tee, we also view critical thinking as an intermediary variable, playing a mediating role in algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity within CPS skills [ 21 , 95 ].

Personal distress indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Fantasy indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Perspective-taking indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Empathic concern is positively related to the CPS skills.

Algorithmic thinking indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Creativity indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Cooperativity indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Critical thinking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Designing the fsQCA configuration model

In this study, a Venn diagram is used to design the fsQCA configuration model (Fig.  1 B), which was used to explore the causal model for improving CPS skills. In the diagram, arrow A represents a combination of perspective-taking, fantasy, and personal distress, and adds configurations that affect the CPS skills through, or including, empathetic concern. Arrow B represents a combination of algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity, and adds configurations that affect the CPS skills through, or including, critical thinking. Arrow C represents the combination of all the variables and represents the complex interaction of these factors to predict the resulting conditions.

Participants

Participants were 136 freshmen and medical majors from a university in southeastern China (‾Xage = 18.47, female = 82.35%, male = 17.65%). The inclusion criterion comprised students who had conducted reflective learning. The exclusion criteria comprised: (1) Students who did not make reflective videos, or (2) students suspected of plagiarizing reflective learning achievements. A total of 163 cases were included in the empirical study of reflective learning, and 136 effective samples were recovered, with an effective recovery rate of 83.44%.

Design and procedure

After receiving appropriate online training, classroom teachers implemented a reflective learning curriculum design among medical students in the autumn of 2021 (Fig.  2 ). Based on the Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Courses, the two rounds of teaching plan lasted a total of 14 weeks was design. In the first round of reflective learning, subjects were asked to read relevant literature, watch relevant video materials, etc., and carry out online learning. They were then asked to record learning videos on their own, and then upload the videos, followed by a double-blind mutual evaluation of learning videos between online students. In the second round of reflective learning, students adjusted their reflective learning according to the feedback from the previous round of mutual evaluation, implemented a second round of deeper material learning exploration, improved their reflective video, and summarized the main points of reflective learning. Teachers evaluated the reflective videos and learning points offline, and students learned and summarized according to the evaluation results. After the end of the entire process, we issued a competency assessment questionnaire to measure learners’ competency levels and the data was collected.

figure 2

Reflective learning process

To measure the constructs under study, existing scales were used (see Table  1 for items associated with each construct and scale reliabilities).

A questionnaire was developed based on the existing mature scale, and the items were slightly adjusted according to the model. The relationship between the retained items and the dimensions was not complementary. Improvement of CPS skills is described as a structure composed of six antecedent variables and two mediating variables with different ways of thinking. The Davis Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was used for personal distress, fantasy, perspective-taking, and empathic concern [ 38 ], and the Computational Thinking Scale (CTS) was used for critical thinking, algorithmic thinking, creativity, problem solving, and cooperativity [ 74 , 96 , 97 ]. We structured it as personal distress (three items), fantasy (three items), and perspective-taking (two items) as ante-dependent variables, and the mediating effect of empathic concern (three items) on CPS skills (three items) was directly and through empathic concern (3 items). Similarly, algorithmic thinking (3 items), creativity (three items), and cooperativity (three items) acted as ante-variables, both directly and through the mediating effect of critical thinking (three items). All items were evaluated using a Likert 5-point scale, 5 = strongly agree, 4 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 2 = disagree, 1 = strongly disagree, and the scores of items in reverse scoring were reversed. Entries for reverse scoring are marked with * in Table  1 . The questionnaire was translated into Chinese and distributed after discussion with experts.

Data analysis

We use multiple methods to analyze the data. First, PLS-SEM was carried out on the data through Smart-PLS 3.0 software to adapt the complex model analysis and explore the impact of various factors [ 48 ].

We measured the characteristics of the structure using internal consistency reliability, convergence validity, and discrimination validity. Internal consistency reliability was measured using the alpha and combinatorial reliability of Cronbach. And we checked the collinearity of the internal model and evaluated the deviation of the method using a variance inflation factor (VIF). According to the research objectives, we tested two models with different paths with significant correlations. The direct predictive effects of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking, as well as the mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking, on CPS skills were tested. A nonparametric, bias-corrected bootstrap with 5,000 subsamples and a 95% confidence interval was used. The structural model was evaluated by R² and by the significance of the estimated value of pathway relationships. The significance of pathway coefficients was evaluated using the bootstrap subsamples, and the structural model was evaluated using 5000 bootstrap subsamples [ 98 ]. R² values of 0.25, 0.50, or 0.75 are considered weak, moderate, and significant, respectively.

Although PLS-SEM can handle both external (measurement) and internal (structural) models [ 98 ], it is limited by symmetry. Therefore, we used fsQCA 3.1 software [ 49 ] to analyze asymmetry and obtain a sufficient causal combination configuration to study the complex relationship between variables more comprehensively and in detail. According to the fsQCA user guide, data calibration, truth table construction, and causal condition analysis are necessary steps in the process of data analysis [ 49 ]. In the first step, we converted the ordinary data into fuzzy sets by setting the original values from the Likert scale, which corresponded to full membership, cross-over anchors, and full non-membership based on Kallmuenzer’s analysis [ 99 ]. The second step is to construct the truth table and generate different combinations of causal conditions that are sufficient to affect the CPS skills by specifying a consistent cutoff value as the natural breakpoint in the consistency and the case number threshold as 1. The third, we analyzed the necessity of all the variables (critical thinking, creativity, algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, empathetic concern, perspective-taking, personal distress and fantasy) to the CPS skills, and the antecedent variables for mediate variables (critical thinking and empathic concern), and the necessity of mediating variables to the outcome variables. It is generally believed that a condition or combination of conditions is “necessary” or “almost always necessary” when the consistency score is higher than 0.9 [ 49 ]. Finally, we use standard analysis to obtain “intermediate solutions” (i.e., partial logical remainders are incorporated into the solution) to identify causal patterns that affect CPS skills.

The result of PLS-SEM

Evaluation of the reflection measurement model.

Except for the perspective-taking, the Cronbach’s alpha in the other dimensions was generally more than 0.7, reaching the standard recommended by Cohen (Table  1 ) [ 100 ]. After examining the external loads in the external model, we observed that most of the loads were more than 0.7, while the PD1 project was still less than 0.7. After checking the Cronbach’s alpha and average variance extracted (AVE), we confirmed that this factor had no negative effect on our research [ 98 ], and was thus retained the project. The sample size of the model is small (less than 300), and the items considered by perspective-taking are 2 (less than 3), so Cronbach’s alpha is easily less than 0.6. The alpha of perspective-taking is more than 0.5, which is still in a slightly plausible range. Therefore, we kept the item of perspective-taking. Secondly, the square root of AVE was greater than 0.5, which accords with the convergence validity [ 101 ]. In addition, we used the Fornell-Larker criteria to evaluate the discriminant validity (Table  2 ).

Evaluation of formative measurement models

The results showed that the VIF of all constructs was lower than the threshold of 3.3 (see Additional file. 1 ) [ 98 ]. In order to further analyze, this study evaluated the quality by blindfolding program (Q 2 ) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The results showed that SRMR = 0.079, not exceeding 0.09 [ 102 ]. The blindfold program showed that Q 2 was greater than 0, which verified the predictive correlation of the research model [ 103 ].

Structural model evaluation

Evaluation of the structural model showed that the R² value was reasonable for exploratory research. Meanwhile, the direct pathway effect of fantasy, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity on CPS skills was not significant ( p  > 0.05), and the pathway effect of personal distress on empathic concern was also not significant ( p  > 0.05). The other variables showed significant influences on CPS skills ( p  < 0.05) (Table  3 ). After deleting the insignificant pathways, we built a structural model between the CPS skills and the influencing factors (critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress) (Fig.  3 ). Compared with the hypothetical model, mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking were observed; however, personal distress only had a direct effect on CPS skills, which was consistent with the previous view that empathic concern and personal distress should be discussed [ 51 ].

figure 3

Path model and partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) estimates

The result of fsQCA

The results of necessity showed that only cooperativity and creativity are necessary conditions for critical thinking (see Additional file. 2 , Additional file. 3 , and Additional file. 4 ).

FsQCA assessed the complex causal combination that led to improved CPS skills (Tables  4 , 5 , 6 and 7 ). The solution provided clues for each different pathway to the result, with all consistency values being higher than 0.8, and most coverage values being between 0.240 and 0.839 [ 104 ].

As shown in Table  4 , there are six approaches to the final model of complex conditions that lead to high CPS skills, among which the top three in terms of coverage are: (1) To achieve high CPS skills through high critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking (consistency = 0.974, coverage = 0.354). (2) Under conditions of high critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, and creativity, combined with high empathic concern, personal distress, and fantasy, the CPS skills can be improved (consistency = 0.950, coverage = 0.352). (3) A high level of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, personal distress, perspective-taking, and fantasy (consistency = 0.950, coverage = 0.340) can promote the improvement of CPS skills.

To examine the mediating effect of empathic concern and critical thinking on the CPS skill, we analyzed the complex causality of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, and empathic concern. The results showed in Table  5 indicated that the complex causal statement of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, and empathic concern is one way, i.e., high perspective-taking and fantasy improves empathic concern skill (consistency = 0.821; coverage = 0.612), which supports the H7b and H7c assumptions in the SEM model. By contrast, the results of analyzing the complex causal relationship of creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking for critical thinking showed that there is a pathway for the complex causal statement of creativity, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, and critical thinking (consistency = 0.867, coverage = 0.760), which will lead to improved critical thinking ability. This supported the hypotheses of H8a, H8b, and H8c in the SEM model.

The results of further analysis of the complex causal relationship between empathic concern and critical thinking for improved CPS skills (Table  7 ) showed that high empathic concern and critical thinking (consistency = 0.890, coverage = 0.550) will lead to improved CPS skills. This supported the H7d and H8d assumptions in the SEM model.

Discussion and conclusion

Theoretical implication.

To improve its ability to deal with complex practical problems, education has been committed to providing teaching measures that can stimulate subjects’ rational and irrational thinking. Healthcare professionals who utilize reflective learning must apply empathetic concern and critical thinking to confront challenges with high-quality solutions. Although previous studies confirmed the positive effects of empathic concern [ 19 ], and critical thinking [ 17 ] on CPS skills through symmetrical analysis, few studies have tested empathic concern and critical thinking at the same time. There is a dearth of studies that specifically investigate the factors that affect CPS skills in the context of reflective learning. And the previous studies on the factors influencing CPS skills mainly adopted traditional symmetric methods (such as regression and SEM), which did not fully capture the complexity behind the factors of influencing CPS skills. For instance, Hwang discovered that collaboration plays a crucial role in problem-solving, whereas communication may not be essential. In contrast, Kocak holds a contrasting perspective [ 21 , 105 ]. The factors affecting the CPS skills are often based on multiple causalities rather than a single causal relationship. Therefore, based on the theory of multi-dimensional empathy [ 38 ] and 21st-century skills [ 21 ], we analyzed the data of 136 medical students undergoing reflective learning using PLS-SEM and fsQCA, and constructed a hypothetical model to examine the relationships between the CPS skills and influence factors (critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, personal distress).

The PLS-SEM results (Table  3 ) showed that a variety of attributes can affect the CPS skills, among which critical thinking and empathic concern play an intermediary role between most antecedents and CPS skills. The fsQCA results partly verified the mediating effect of critical thinking and empathetic concern (Tables  5 and 6 ). In the PLS-SEM results (Table  3 ), personal distress was identified to directly affect the CPS skills; and the effect of personal distress on empathic concern was not shown in the fsQCA solution (Table  5 ), which proved that personal distress directly affects the CPS skills without the intermediary of empathetic concern. This result is similar to that of Jeon [ 106 ]; however, he believed that there is a negative correlation between personal distress and problem solving, which might be related to the different learning patterns (reflective learning) used in this study. Personal distress is a necessary process in reflective learning because the motivation of prosocial behavior eases our uncomfortable state of mind by reducing the disgusting and awakening cues sent out by others [ 107 ]. Reflection urges us to face these emotions and draw lessons from them. Hoffman noted that excessive personal distress can turn others-oriented motivation into self-directed motivation, thus reducing the occurrence of prosocial behavior [ 54 ], which emphasizes the differential treatment of personal distress in different learning modes. In addition, perspective-taking was identified to affect the CPS skills directly (Table  3 ; C1 in Table  4 ) and indirectly (Table  3 ; C1 in Table  5 ). Therefore, some of the results obtained from fSQCA validated the conclusions of PLS-SEM to some extent (Table  8 ).

The fsQCA results provided more configuration solutions of complex causality, which extended the results of PLS-SEM and further revealed the complexity of affecting the CPS skills. For example, the fsQCA results in Table  7 not only proved the mediating effect of empathetic concern and critical thinking, but also suggest that they work together to affect the CPS skills. This demonstrates that CPS skills are impacted by both rational and irrational thinking, and positive emotions play a critical role in fostering CPS skills [ 108 ]. In Table  4 cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, critical thinking, and personal distress all appear in forward solutions with high coverage (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ). This suggested that personal distress, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, and critical thinking can be regarded as the core conditions to affect the CPS skills, and these conditions make an important contribution in the context of reflective learning. Researches by Chen [ 77 ], Garrett [ 75 ], Geisinger [ 70 ] and Ellis [ 17 ] respectively believed that collaboration, creativity, algorithmic thinking and critical thinking play an important role in CPS skills, and Sze [ 109 ] believed that personal distress could have a positive impact on prosocial behavior and altruism. The studies above provide are similar to our perspective.

Critical thinking is not only one of the basic skills in the 21st Century but also a key ability in reflective learning [ 110 , 111 ]. The process of questioning and reorganizing critical thinking is key to reflective learning. The complex structure of the problem-solving process requires critical thinking skills to find different solutions [ 30 , 112 , 113 , 114 ]. Table  4 higher coverage solutions (C1, C2, C3, NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ) and Table  7 higher coverage solutions (C1, NC1 in Table  7 ) showed that critical thinking ability training is helpful to improve a subject’s CPS skills. By contrast, a lack of critical thinking training is not conducive to improving CPS skills (NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ; NC1 in Table  7 ). Critical thinking is widely considered to be a competency closely linked to CPS skills [ 29 ], and our study approves this perspective. Moreover, cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking also appear in the forward solutions with high coverage (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ), combined with the mediating effect of critical thinking on cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, and the CPS skills. It is logical that the antecedents of critical thinking, such as cooperativity, creativity and algorithmic thinking, also play a positive role in the CPS skills. The result is similar to the findings of Özgenel [ 115 ], who believed that critical thinking and creative thinking affected problem-solving skill through decision-making style. These results suggested that we should pay attention to the cultivation of a critical thinking ability, especially through the cultivation of cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking, which positively and significantly improve a subject’s ability to solve complex problems.

Empathetic concern relationship with the complex configuration between its antecedent variables provides new ideas and insights to improve our ability to solve complex problems. Empathic concern, as a key factor of prosocial behavior [ 43 ], is also of positive significance to CPS skills in this study. The higher coverage solutions (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ; NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ; C1 in Table  7 ; NC1 in Table  7 ) showed that training in empathic concern is beneficial to improve a subject’s CPS skills, while a lack of empathic concern training is not conducive to improving CPS skills (NC1, NC2 in Table  4 ; NC1 in Table  7 ). By contrast, fantasy (C2, C3 in Table  4 ) and perspective-taking (C1, C3 in Table  4 ) appeared among the forward solutions with higher coverage in Table  4 . Combined with the mediating effect among empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and the CPS skills, it is not difficult for us to understand that the empathic concern antecedent variable: fantasy, and perspective-taking, also have positive significance for the CPS skills. This result aligns with the research findings of Hashmi [ 57 ] and Davenport [ 66 ]. Moreover, in the absence of empathetic concern, the pathway support of the combination of fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress for CPS skills also verified this positive significance from the other side (C3 in Table  4 ). However, personal distress (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ) appears independently in the forward solution, with high coverage in Table  4 , which verifies the direct effect of personal distress on the CPS skills, which was consistent with the results of PLSSEM. These further confirmed the theory of Dorner and Funke, who suggested that complex and dynamic non-routine situations across different domains require a collection of self-regulating psychological processes and a creative combination of knowledge and strategies, and is influenced by motivation and emotion, especially in a high-stakes environment[24]. In addition, according to the observation of the reverse solution of Table  4 , the combination of negative perspectivetaking and negative personal distress will be conducive to the low-level CPS skills (NC1, NC2, NC3, NC5 in Table  4 ). Interestingly, fantasy appeared not only in the forward solution with high coverage (C2, C3 in Table  4 ), but also in the inverse solution with high coverage (NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ), which seemed to suggest that the contribution of fantasy to improving CPS skills is neutral, which requires further research.

Consistent with the principle of causal asymmetry, fsQCA suggested that solutions generated by the same attributes in different areas might have the opposite impact on CPS skills, depending on how they combine or interact with other attributes. The lack or negation of some positive factors will lead to improved CPS skills, while the existence of some negative factors might also lead to similar results, depending on how they are configured with the other factors. For example, solution 4 in Table  4 shows that in the absence of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy, a combination of empathetic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking could also have a positive effect on the improvement of CPS skills. There is a paucity of literature exploring the effects of empathetic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking on CPS skills under conditions of low levels of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy. These insights provide new ideas for exploring improvements in CPS skills. Although PLS-SEM can verify the predetermined relationship between previous factors and the results of interest, it cannot provide these insights.

In addition, considering the complex nature of problem-solving skill under the condition of reflective learning, it is necessary to check the linear and nonlinear relationships between structures to fully understand the strategies and methods to improve CPS skills. In this study, as an ideal approach, PLS-SEM was used to identify the linear (symmetric) causal relationship between the improvement of CPS skills and influence factors. The fSQCA was used to identify the nonlinear (asymmetric), heterogeneous, and dynamic interactions between antecedents and behavioral results. The fSQCA improved identifying sufficient causal conditions for outcomes. The comprehensive application of PLS-SEM and fsQCA helped capture complex multiple causalities in the improvement of CPS skills, which makes theoretical contributions in terms of analytical techniques.

Practical implications

Aquino believes that the implementation of reflective learning strategies is conducive to the improvement of CPS skills [ 116 ], which is of practical significance for the design of learning strategies and training tools, including reflective learning. The PLS-SEM results showed that perspective-taking, as an important condition for affected CPS skills, not only plays a role through the intermediary effect of empathetic concern, but also directly affects the CPS skills. The researchers and learners can train subjects to think for others in the form of team communication and exchange of views. For the sake of others, it is necessary to think about CPS skills solutions from multiple angles and more comprehensively, by thinking about problems from the standpoint and perspective of others. Therefore, it is necessary to adopt evidence-based strategies for training to improve the CPS skills.

While the fsQCA results confirmed the PLS-SEM results, in turn its complex configuration helps researchers and learners to make more informed decisions about learning methods to improve CPS skills. The derived pathways indicated that there is more than one causal configuration that can improve CPS skills, and how to improve depends on a combination of attributes. For example, our results showed that the high level of critical thinking and its antecedent attributes, combined with the high level of empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking, will lead to improvement of the CPS skills. The lack of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy, which to some extent emphasizes the utility of empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking (Table  4 solution 4), make it necessary to pay attention to training medical students in empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking using reflective learning. Aligned with our own research, it was acknowledged reflective learning as a potent method to enhance empathy [ 37 ]. Medical reflection should focus on cultivating the ability to speculate on materials and self-views, and at the same time, understand decision-making from the situation of others and feel the emotions of others to trigger empathy. Improvement of CPS skills should not only emphasize the reduction of personal distress, but also should look at the role of personal distress critically. At the same time, it also reminds us that we should fully consider the training situation of the subjects in the design of learning strategies. Critical thinking and its antecedents are regarded as the key solutions in fsQCA, which suggests that we can focus on the reflective learning mode when we train subjects for critical thinking, creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking. We should also consciously use this kind of thinking to solve problems in the process of reflective learning. In the design of other learning strategies, training in critical thinking ability and its antecedent variables, cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking, can effectively help subjects to improve their CPS skills.

Based on our understanding of how empathic concern and critical thinking work together to improve the CPS skills, we suggest that real and complex problems in life be taken as examples in the choice of reflective teaching strategies, to involve a series of related skills and characteristics, and fully exercise the two modes of thinking. This is because, in reflective learning, subjects internalize the thinking skills taught by others into their own thinking skills, cultivating the ability to monitor and reflect on the whole problem-solving process, and helping subjects to extract useful strategies, experiences, and patterns into their cognitive structure, thereby improving their CPS skills and accumulating more experience for possible intuitive thinking. This is more suitable for problems based on real-life, which is in line with the medical learning problem-based learning and case-based learning models.

In this study, a hypothetical model of the relationship between the CPS skills and influencing factors (critical thinking, cooperation, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress) was constructed and validated. The model confirmed the mediating effect of critical thinking and empathic concern on the CPS skills, the direct effect of personal distress, and the direct and indirect effect of perspective-taking on the CPS skills. Besides, fsQCA results provided a variety of configurations that enhanced the improvement of CPS skills. The findings not only enriched the theoretical system of affecting CPS skills, but also provided practical guidance for the development of learning strategies and assessment tools aimed at improving CPS skills.

Limitations and future research

Although this study enriches the theoretical and practical knowledge concerning the relationship between CPS skills and critical thinking, empathic concern, and other variables, it also has some limitations. First, the subjects were beginners in terms of reflective learning under the guidance of teachers, and lack experience in reflective learning, which might affect the accuracy and applicability of variables to some extent. In future research, we will improve these shortcomings, practice reflective learning practices in more subjects, and validate the model in a broader learning strategy, which would be very meaningful. Second, based on the model of this study, it is necessary to enrich the paths and develop a variety of learning and training tools to improve the CPS skills in the future research. The development of assessment tools for factors related to the measurement of CPS skills will facilitate targeted training and realize personalized learning practice guidance.

Data Availability

The data sets used and / or analyzed in this study have not been made public. If there is a reasonable need, they can be obtained from and provided by the corresponding author of this article.

Abbreviations

Complex problem-solving

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development

Partial least square structural equation modeling

  • Fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis

Average variance extracted

Variance inflation factor

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

Computational Thinking Scale

Structural equation modeling.

Co-operation OfE, Development. PISA 2012 results: Creative problem solving: Students’ skills in tackling real-life problems (Volume V). In.: OECD Publishing Pisa; 2014.

Amirian K. P-833-To study the effect of problem-solving skill education on first year high school male students in songhor koliayi township in academic year. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(S1):1–1. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0924-9338(12)75000-9 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Ayalew MM. Multilevel analysis of entrepreneurial intention of engineering graduating students in Ethiopia. J Appl Econ. 2021;24(1):366–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/15140326.2021.1949256 1992.

Elliott TR. Social problem-solving abilities and adjustment to recent-onset spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol. 1999;44(4):315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0090-5550.44.4.315 .

Woods DR, Hrymak AN, Marshall RR, Wood PE, Crowe CM, Hoffman TW, Wright JD, Taylor PA, Woodhouse KA, Bouchard CK. Developing problem solving skills: the McMaster problem solving program. J Eng Educ. 1997;86(2):75–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2168-9830.1997.tb00270.x .

Lohman MC, Finkelstein M. Designing groups in problem-based learning to promote problem-solving skill and self-directedness. Instr Sci. 2000;28(4):291–307. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003927228005 .

Puplampu V. Nursing students’ and faculty members’ experiences of comfort during transition to context-based learning. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh. 2017;14(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.teln.2021.02.008 .

Chen SL, Huang TW, Liao IC, Liu C. Development and validation of the simulation learning effectiveness inventory. J Adv Nurs. 2015;71(10):2444–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/jan.12707 .

Miller GP, Molina-Ray C. Beyond technology dependence: critically reflexive thinking in higher education. J Leadersh Stud. 2010;4(1):74–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/jls.20156 .

Gadbury-Amyot CC, Godley LW, Nelson JW Jr. Measuring the level of reflective ability of predoctoral dental students: early outcomes in an e‐portfolio reflection. J Dent Educ. 2019;83(3):275–80. https://doi.org/10.21815/JDE.019.025 .

Bernack C, Holzäpfel L, Leuders T, Renkl A. INITIATING CHANGE ON PRE-SERVICE TECHERS’BELIEFS IN A REFLEXIVE PROBLEM SOLVING COURSEi. CURRENT STATE OF RESEARCH ON MATHEMATICAL BELIEFS XVI 2011:27.

Kellogg RT. The psychology of writing. Oxford University Press; 1999.

Atkins S, Murphy K. Reflection: a review of the literature. J Adv Nurs. 1993;18(8):1188–92. https://doi.org/10.1046/j1365-2648199318081188x .

Lau AKL, Chuk KC, Wei So WK. Reflective practise in clinical teaching. Nurs Health Sci. 2002;4(4):201–8. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1442-2018.2002.00121.x .

Illeris K. Læring (Nordgård, Y.). In.: Oslo: Gyldendal akademisk; 2012.

Barton G, Ryan M. Multimodal approaches to reflective teaching and assessment in higher education. High Educ Res Dev. 2014;33(3):409–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2013.841650 .

Ellis S, Carette B, Anseel F, Lievens F. Systematic reflection: implications for learning from failures and successes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(1):67–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413504106 .

Bulman C, Schutz S. Reflective practice in nursing. John Wiley & Sons; 2013.

Whittock T. Reflexive teaching, reflexive learning. Teach High Educ. 1997;2(2):93–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/1356251970020201 .

Nuninger W, Picardi C, Goy A, Petrone G. Multi-perspective concept mapping in a digital integrated learning environment: Promote active learning through shared perspectives. Educational Technology and the New World of Persistent Learning.edn.: IGI Global; 2019: 114–44.

Kocak O, Coban M, Aydin A, Cakmak N. The mediating role of critical thinking and cooperativity in the 21st century skills of higher education students. Think Skills Creativity. 2021;42:100967. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100967 .

Newell A, Simon HA. Human problem solving. Volume 104. Prentice-hall Englewood Cliffs, NJ; 1972.

Buchner A. Basic topics and approaches to the study of complex problem solving. Complex problem solving: The European perspective 1995:27–63.

Dörner D, Funke J. Complex problem solving: what it is and what it is not. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1153. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01153 .

Fischer A, Neubert JC. The multiple faces of complex problems: a model of problem solving competency and its implications for training and assessment. J Dynamic Decis Mak. 2015;1:6–6. https://doi.org/10.11588/jddm.2015.1.23945 .

Funke J, Fischer A, Holt DV. Competencies for complexity: problem solving in the twenty-first century. Assessment and teaching of 21st century skills.edn.: Springer; 2018: 41–53.

Akpinar Ö. The Effect of Empathy levels of Female School of Physical Education and Sports Students on Problem solving skill levels. Asian J Educ Train. 2020;6(3):406–11.

Doleck T, Bazelais P, Lemay DJ, Saxena A, Basnet RB. Algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, creativity, critical thinking, and problem solving: exploring the relationship between computational thinking skills and academic performance. J Computers Educ 2017, 4(4):355–69 https://doi.org/10.1007/s40692-017-0090-9 .

Ananiadoui K, Claro M. 21st century skills and competences for new millennium learners in OECD countries. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1787/19939019 .

Daud D, Santoso RH. Device learning development using Cabri 3D with problem-solving method based on oriented critical thinking ability and learning achievements of junior high school students. In: 5th Asia Pasific Education Conference (AECON 2018): 2018: Atlantis Press; 2018: 23–28.

Benham MK. The practitioner-scholars’ view of school change: a case-based approach to teaching and learning. Teach Teacher Educ. 1996;12(2):119–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/0742-051X(95)00026-G .

Argelagos E, Pifarré M. Improving information problem solving skills in secondary education through embedded instruction. Comput Hum Behav. 2012;28(2):515–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.024 .

Kim KT. The structural relationship among digital literacy, learning strategies, and core competencies among south korean college students. Educational sciences: theory and practice. 2019;19(2):3–21. https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2019.2.001 .

Wink MN, LaRusso MD, Smith RL. Teacher empathy and students with problem behaviors: examining teachers’ perceptions, responses, relationships, and burnout. Psychol Sch. 2021;58(8):1575–96. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22516 .

Leppma M. The effect of loving-kindness meditation on empathy, perceived social support, and problem-solving appraisal in counseling students. 2011.

Zheng S, Masuda T, Matsunaga M, Noguchi Y, Ohtsubo Y, Yamasue H, Ishii K. Cultural differences in social support seeking: the mediating role of empathic concern. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(12):e0262001.

Imperato A, Strano-Paul L. Impact of reflection on empathy and emotional intelligence in third-year medical students. Acad Psychiatry. 2021;45(3):350–3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40596-020-01371-1 .

Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. 1980.

Kim H, Han S. Does personal distress enhance empathic interaction or block it? Pers Indiv Differ. 2018;124:77–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.005 .

Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. TheScientificWorldJOURNAL. 2006;6:1146–63. https://doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2006.221 .

Batson CD, O’Quin K, Fultz J, Vanderplas M, Isen AM. Influence of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1983;45(3):706. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.706 .

Nomura K, Akai S. Empathy with fictional stories: reconsideration of the fantasy scale of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(1):304–14. https://doi.org/10.2466/02070911PR01101304-314 .

Vaish A. Empathy and prosocial behavior. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21816-6 .

Stocks EL, Lishner DA, Waits BL, Downum EM. I’m embarrassed for you: the effect of valuing and perspective taking on empathic embarrassment and empathic concern. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2011;41(1):1–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00699.x .

Archer RL, Diaz-Loving R, Gollwitzer PM, Davis MH, Foushee HC. The role of dispositional empathy and social evaluation in the empathic mediation of helping. 1981. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.4.786 .

Ampuero D, Miranda CE, Delgado LE, Goyen S, Weaver S. Empathy and critical thinking: primary students solving local environmental problems through outdoor learning. J Adventure Educ Outdoor Learn. 2015;15(1):64–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/14729679.2013.848817 .

Moitra M, Buch R, Damor R. Problem solving skills among adolescents in Surat city: a reality check. Natl J Community Med. 2019;10:571–4.

Google Scholar  

Ringle CM, Wende S, Becker J-M. SmartPLS 3. Boenningstedt: SmartPLS GmbH 2015, 584.

Ragin CC, Strand SI, Rubinson C. User’s guide to fuzzy-set/qualitative comparative analysis. Univ Arizona. 2008;87:1–87.

Carrera P, Oceja L, Caballero A, Muñoz D, López-Pérez B, Ambrona T. I feel so sorry! Tapping the joint influence of empathy and personal distress on helping behavior. Motivation and Emotion. 2013;37(2):335–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9302-9 .

Eisenberg N, Eggum ND. Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. The social neuroscience of empathy 2009, 6(2009):71–830. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262012973.003.0007 .

Rodríguez-Nogueira Ó, Moreno-Poyato AR, Álvarez-Álvarez MJ, Pinto-Carral A. Significant socio-emotional learning and improvement of empathy in physiotherapy students through service learning methodology: a mixed methods research. Nurse Educ Today. 2020;90:104437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2020.104437 .

Reynolds F. Communication and clinical effectiveness in rehabilitation. Elsevier Health Sciences; 2004.

Hoffman ML. Empathy and moral development: implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press; 2001.

Joireman J. Empathy and the self-absorption paradox II: self-rumination and self-reflection as mediators between shame, guilt, and empathy. Self and Identity. 2004;3(3):225–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500444000038 .

Stanovich K. Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford University Press; 2011.

Hashmi S, Vanderwert RE, Paine AL, Gerson SA. Doll play prompts social thinking and social talking: representations of internal state language in the brain. Dev Sci. 2021;e13163. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13163 .

Van Reet J. Pretense, Imagination, and Fantasy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-818872-9.00060-1 .

Zipes J. Why fantasy matters too much. CLCWeb: Comp Literature Cult. 2008;10(4). https://doi.org/10.1353/jae.0.0039 .

Brown MM, Thibodeau RB, Pierucci JM, Gilpin AT. Supporting the development of empathy: the role of theory of mind and fantasy orientation. Soc Dev. 2017;26(4):951–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12232 .

Weibel D, Martarelli CS, Häberli D, Mast FW. The fantasy questionnaire: a measure to assess creative and imaginative fantasy. J Pers Assess. 2018;100(4):431–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1331913 .

Bauer RH, Gilpin AT, Thibodeau-Nielsen RB. Executive functions and imaginative play: Exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 2021, 25:100165 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165 .

Becker R. Empathie. Potenziale entdecken.edn.: Springer; 2016: 65–84.

Falk DR, Johnson DW. The effects of perspective-taking and egocentrism on problem solving in heterogeneous and homogeneous groups. J Soc Psychol. 1977;102(1):63–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1977.9713241 .

Johnson DW. Role reversal: A summary and review of the research. Int J Group Tensions 1971.

Davenport SW, Rentsch JR. Managing conflict through team member schema accuracy: a fresh perspective on perspective taking. J Theoretical Social Psychol. 2021;5(4):449–63. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.110 .

Bethune C, Brown JB. Residents’ use of case-based reflection exercises. Can Fam Physician. 2007;53(3):470–6.

Hsu C-C, Wang T-I. Applying game mechanics and student-generated questions to an online puzzle-based game learning system to promote algorithmic thinking skills. Comput Educ. 2018;121:73–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.02.002 .

Gürbüz H, Evlioğlu B, Erol ÇS, Gülseçen H, Gülseçen S. What’s the weather like today?”: a computer game to develop algorithmic thinking and problem solving skills of primary school pupils. Educ Inform Technol. 2017;22(3):1133–47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-016-9478-9 .

Geisinger KF. 21st century skills: what are they and how do we assess them? Appl Measur Educ. 2016;29(4):245–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/08957347.2016.1209207 .

Futschek G, Moschitz J. Developing algorithmic thinking by inventing and playing algorithms. Proceedings of the 2010 constructionist approaches to creative learning, thinking and education: Lessons for the 21st century (constructionism 2010) 2010:1–10.

Runco MA, Pritzker SR. Encyclopedia of creativity. Academic press; 2020.

Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. The concept of creativity: prospects and paradigms. Handb creativity. 1999;1:3–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807916.003 .

Korkmaz Ö, Çakir R, Özden MY. A validity and reliability study of the computational thinking scales (CTS). Comput Hum Behav. 2017;72:558–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.005 .

Garrett RM. Issues in science education: problem-solving, creativity and originality. Int J Sci Educ. 1987;9(2):125–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069870090201 .

Chen C, Kasof J, Himsel AJ, Greenberger E, Dong Q, Xue G. Creativity in drawings of geometric shapes: a cross-cultural examination with the consensual assessment technique. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2002;33(2):171–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033002004 .

Chen C-C, Swan K. Using innovative and scientifically-based debate to Build e-Learning Community. Online Learn. 2020;24(3):67–80.

Claes E, Schrooten M, McLaughlin H, Csoba J. Community service learning in complex urban settings: challenges and opportunities for social work education. Social Work Education 2021:1–19 https://doi.org/10.1080/02615479.2021.1948003 .

Nazirova G, Yulchiboeva D, Khaydarov O. The main factors of formation and development of Professional competence of Teachers of Preschool Educational Organizations. REVISTA GEINTEC-GESTAO INOVACAO E TECNOLOGIAS. 2021;11(3):1698–708.

Gibbs G. Learning by doing: a guide to teaching and learning methods. Further Education Unit; 1988.

Kelly M. Enhancing Cognitive Empathy. North Carolina State University; 2021.

Pang Y, Song C, Ma C. Effect of different types of empathy on prosocial behavior: Gratitude as mediator. Front Psychol 2022, 13.

Siem B. The relationship between empathic concern and perceived personal costs for helping and how it is affected by similarity perceptions. J Soc Psychol 2021:1–20 https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2021.1996321 .

Underwood B, Moore B. Perspective-taking and altruism. Psychol Bull. 1982;91(1):143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.143 .

Fatma A, Polat S, Kashimi T. Relationship between the problem-solving skills and empathy skills of operating room nurses. J Nurs Res. 2020;28(2):e75. https://doi.org/10.1097/jnr.0000000000000357 .

Contreras JA, Edwards-Maddox S, Hall A, Lee MA. Effects of reflective practice on baccalaureate nursing students’ stress, anxiety and competency: an integrative review. Worldviews on Evidence‐Based Nursing. 2020;17(3):239–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/wvn.12438 .

Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM, Holmgren HG, Davis EJ, Collier KM, Memmott-Elison MK, Hawkins AJ. A meta-analysis of prosocial media on prosocial behavior, aggression, and empathic concern: a multidimensional approach. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(2):331.

O’Brien E, Konrath SH, Grühn D, Hagen AL. Empathic concern and perspective taking: Linear and Quadratic Effects of Age across the Adult Life Span. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2013;68(2):168–75. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs055 .

Gülseven Z, Kumru A, Carlo G, De Guzman MR. The roles of perspective taking, empathic concern, and Prosocial Moral reasoning in the self-reported prosocial behaviors of Filipino and turkish young adults. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022120968265 .

Wrigglesworth S. Understanding reflective practice. Nurs Standard (2014+). 2016;31(8):72. https://doi.org/10.7748/ns.31.8.72.s48 .

Amerson R, Livingston WG. Reflexive photography: an alternative method for documenting the learning process of cultural competence. J Transcult Nurs. 2014;25(2):202–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043659613515719 .

Davies SM, Reitmaier AB, Smith LR, Mangan-Danckwart D. Capturing intergenerativity: the use of student reflective journals to identify learning within an undergraduate course in gerontological nursing. J Nurs Educ. 2013;52(3):139–49. https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120213-01 .

Stocker M, Burmester M, Allen M. Optimisation of simulated team training through the application of learning theories: a debate for a conceptual framework. BMC Med Educ. 2014;14(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-14-69 .

Parrish DR, Crookes K. Designing and implementing reflective practice programs–key principles and considerations. Nurse Educ Pract. 2014;14(3):265–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nepr.2013.08.002 .

Tee KN, Leong KE, Abdul Rahim SS. The mediating effects of critical thinking skills on motivation factors for mathematical reasoning ability. Asia-Pacific Educ Researcher. 2018;27:373–82.

Pellas N, Vosinakis S. The effect of simulation games on learning computer programming: a comparative study on high school students’ learning performance by assessing computational problem-solving strategies. Educ Inform Technol. 2018;23(6):2423–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-018-9724-4 .

Sırakaya M, Alsancak Sırakaya D, Korkmaz Ö. The impact of STEM attitude and thinking style on computational thinking determined via Structural equation modeling. J Sci Edu Technol. 2020;29(4):561–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-020-09836-6 .

Hair JF Jr, Hult GTM, Ringle CM, Sarstedt M. A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (. PLS-SEM): Sage publications;; 2021.

Kallmuenzer A, Kraus S, Peters M, Steiner J, Cheng C-F. Entrepreneurship in tourism firms: a mixed-methods analysis of performance driver configurations. Tour Manag. 2019;74:319–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2019.04.002 .

Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (revised ed.). In.: New York: Academic Press; 1977.

Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res. 1981;18(1):39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800104 .

Henseler J, Dijkstra TK, Sarstedt M, Ringle CM, Diamantopoulos A, Straub DW, Ketchen DJ Jr, Hair JF, Hult GTM, Calantone RJ. Common beliefs and reality about PLS: Comments on Rönkkö and Evermann (2013). Organizational research methods 2014, 17(2):182–209.

Chin WW. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Mod methods Bus Res. 1998;295(2):295–336.

Woodside AG. Moving beyond multiple regression analysis to algorithms: calling for adoption of a paradigm shift from symmetric to asymmetric thinking in data analysis and crafting theory. Volume 66. Elsevier; 2013. pp. 463–72.

Hwang G-J, Lai C-L, Liang J-C, Chu H-C, Tsai C-C. A long-term experiment to investigate the relationships between high school students’ perceptions of mobile learning and peer interaction and higher-order thinking tendencies. Education Tech Research Dev. 2018;66:75–93.

Jeong H. Critical thinking disposition, problem solving process, and empathy among nursing students. Adv Sci Technol Lett. 2015;103:44–8.

Batson CD. Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? Advances in experimental social psychology. Volume 20,edn.: Elsevier; 1987: 65–122.

Isen AM. Some ways in which positive affect influences decision making and problem solving. Handb emotions. 2008;3:548–73.

Sze JA, Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Levenson RW. Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1129.

Boyaci S, Atalay N. A Scale Development for 21st Century Skills of Primary School students: a validity and reliability study. Int J Instruction. 2016;9(1):133–48. https://doi.org/10.12973/iji.2016.9111a .

Herout L. Reflexion of the Methods and Forms of University Education in Economy Oriented Study Programmes in the Czech Republic. In: ICERI2015 Proceedings: 8th annual International Conference of Education, Research and Innovation : 2015; 2015.

Aein F, Hosseini R, Naseh L, Safdari F, Banaian S. The effect of problem-solving-based interprofessional learning on critical thinking and satisfaction with learning of nursing and midwifery students. J Educ Health Promotion. 2020;9. https://doi.org/10.4103/jehp.jehp_640_19 .

Giannakopoulos P, Buckley S. Do problem solving, critical thinking and creativity play a role in knowledge management? A theoretical mathematics perspective. In: Proceedings of the 10 th European Conference on Knowledge Management : 2009; 2009: 327–337.

GOGUS A, Göğüş NG, BAHADIR E. Intersections between critical thinking skills and reflective thinking skills toward problem solving. Pamukkale Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi. 2019;49:1–19. https://doi.org/10.9779/pauefd.526407 .

Özgenel M. Modeling the relationships between school administrators’ creative and critical thinking dispositions with decision making styles and problem solving skills. Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice. 2018;18(3). https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2018.3.0068 .

Aquino HI, Ching DA. Effects of Reflective Learning Resource Material on Achievement of Mathematics Learning Outcome. Int J Educational Manage Dev Stud, 3(1):132–48 https://doi.org/10.53378/352866 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the teachers and students who participated (including the teachers and students of Shulan International Medical College, Zhejiang Shuren University, China), and SmartPLS 3.0 and fsQCA 3.1 for their assistance in analysis of data of this process.

This study was funded by the Provincial Industry-University Cooperation Collaborative Education Project (NO.318 [2022] of the Zhejiang Development Reform Society), the Scientific and technological Innovation activity Plan and New Seedling Talent Plan for College students in Zhejiang Province in 2023, the First-class curriculum project of Zhejiang Province of China (NO.195 [2022] of the Zhejiang Education Office Letter, NO.352 [2022] of the Zhejiang Education Office Letter), the First batch of ideological and political demonstration courses of Zhejiang Province of China (NO.47 [2021] of the Zhejiang Education Letter), and the High-level pre-level program of Zhejiang Shuren University in 2019.

Author information

Ying Wang and Ze-Ling Xu contributed equally to this work.

Authors and Affiliations

Shulan International Medical College, Zhejiang Shuren University, Hangzhou, 310015, China

Ying Wang, Ze-Ling Xu, Jia-Yao Lou & Ke-Da Chen

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

ZLX and JYL conceived the project with the input of YW and KDC. YW, ZLX, and JYL collected and analyzed the relevant data for this study. YW and ZLX are the main authors of this study. All the authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ke-Da Chen .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

Ethical approval was obtained and document informed consent was waived by the Ethics Committee of Zhejiang Shuren University (202201016). All procedures in this study adhered to the World Medical Association (WMA) Declaration of Helsinki (2013) ethical guidelines.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Wang, Y., Xu, ZL., Lou, JY. et al. Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective learning: results from partial least square structural equation modeling and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis. BMC Med Educ 23 , 382 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04326-w

Download citation

Received : 26 April 2022

Accepted : 05 May 2023

Published : 25 May 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04326-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Reflective learning
  • Complex problem-solving skills
  • Partial least squares structural equation modeling

BMC Medical Education

ISSN: 1472-6920

what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Logo for Digital Press at Collier Library

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

9 Chapter 9. Problem-Solving

Lightbulb icon

CHAPTER 9: PROBLEM SOLVING  

Chesspieces

How do we achieve our goals when the solution is not immediately obvious? What mental blocks are likely to get in our way, and how can we leverage our prior knowledge to solve novel problems?

CHAPTER 9 LICENSE AND ATTRIBUTION

Source: Multiple authors. Memory. In Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. Wikibooks. Retrieved from https://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/ Cognitive_Psychology_and_Cognitive_Neuroscience

Wikibooks are licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License.

Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience is licensed under the GNU Free Documentation License.

Condensed from original version. American spellings used. Content added or changed to reflect American perspective and references. Context and transitions added throughout. Substantially edited, adapted, and (in some parts) rewritten for clarity and course relevance.

Cover photo by Pixabay on Pexels.

Knut is sitting at his desk, staring at a blank paper in front of him, and nervously playing with a pen in his right hand. Just a few hours left to hand in his essay and he has not written a word. All of a sudden he smashes his fist on the table and cries out: “I need a plan!”

Knut is confronted with something every one of us encounters in his daily life: he has a problem, and he does not know how to solve it. But what exactly is a problem? Are there strategies to solve problems? These are just a few of the questions we want to answer in this chapter.

We begin our chapter by giving a short description of what psychologists regard as a problem. Afterward we will discuss different approaches towards problem solving, starting with gestalt psychologists and ending with modern search strategies connected to artificial intelligence. In addition we will also consider how experts solve problems.

The most basic definition of a problem is any given situation that differs from a desired goal. This definition is very useful for discussing problem solving in terms of evolutionary adaptation, as it allows us to understand every aspect of (human or animal) life as a problem. This includes issues like finding food in harsh winters, remembering where you left your provisions, making decisions about which way to go, learning, repeating and varying all kinds of complex movements, and so on. Though all of these problems were of crucial importance during the human evolutionary process, they are by no means solved exclusively by humans. We find an amazing variety of different solutions for these problems in nature (just consider, for example, the way a bat hunts its prey compared to a spider). We will mainly focus on problems that are not solved by animals or evolution; we will instead focus on abstract problems, such as playing chess. Furthermore, we will not consider problems that have an obvious solution. For example, imagine Knut decides to take a sip of coffee from the mug next to his right hand. He does not even have to think about how to do this. This is not because the situation itself is trivial (a robot capable of recognizing the mug, deciding whether it is full, then grabbing it and moving it to Knut’s mouth would be a highly complex machine) but because in the context of all possible situations it is so trivial that it no longer is a problem our consciousness needs to be bothered with. The problems we will discuss in the following all need some conscious effort, though some seem to be solved without us being able to say how exactly we got to the solution. We will often find that the strategies we use to solve these problems are applicable to more basic problems, too.

Non-trivial, abstract problems can be divided into two groups: well-defined problems and ill- defined problems.

WELL-DEFINED PROBLEMS

For many abstract problems, it is possible to find an algorithmic solution. We call problems well-defined if they can be properly formalized, which involves the following properties:

•        The problem has a clearly defined given state. This might be the line-up of a chess game, a given formula you have to solve, or the set-up of the towers of Hanoi game (which we will discuss later).

•        There is a finite set of operators, that is, rules you may apply to the given state. For the chess game, e.g., these would be the rules that tell you which piece you may move to which position.

•        Finally, the problem has a clear goal state: The equations is resolved to x, all discs are moved to the right stack, or the other player is in checkmate.

A problem that fulfils these requirements can be implemented algorithmically. Therefore many well-defined problems can be very effectively solved by computers, like playing chess.

ILL-DEFINED PROBLEMS

Though many problems can be properly formalized, there are still others where this is not the case. Good examples for this are all kinds of tasks that involve creativity, and, generally speaking, all problems for which it is not possible to clearly define a given state and a goal state. Formalizing a problem such as “Please paint a beautiful picture” may be impossible.

Still, this is a problem most people would be able to approach in one way or the other, even if the result may be totally different from person to person. And while Knut might judge that picture X is gorgeous, you might completely disagree.

The line between well-defined and ill-defined problems is not always neat: ill-defined problems often involve sub-problems that can be perfectly well-defined. On the other hand, many everyday problems that seem to be completely well-defined involve — when examined in detail — a great amount of creativity and ambiguity. Consider Knut’s fairly ill-defined task of writing an essay: he will not be able to complete this task without first understanding the text he has to write about. This step is the first subgoal Knut has to solve. In this example, an ill-defined problem involves a well-defined sub-problem

RESTRUCTURING: THE GESTALTIST APPROACH

One dominant approach to problem solving originated from Gestalt psychologists in the 1920s. Their understanding of problem solving emphasizes behavior in situations requiring relatively novel means of attaining goals and suggests that problem solving involves a process called restructuring. With a Gestalt approach, two main questions have to be considered to understand the process of problem solving: 1) How is a problem represented in a person’s mind?, and 2) How does solving this problem involve a reorganization or restructuring of this representation?

HOW IS A PROBLEM REPRESENTED IN THE MIND?

In current research internal and external representations are distinguished: an internal representation is one held in memory, and which has to be retrieved by cognitive processes, while an external representation exists in the environment, such like physical objects or symbols whose information can be picked up and processed by the perceptual system.

Generally speaking, problem representations are models of the situation as experienced by the solver. Representing a problem means to analyze it and split it into separate components, including objects, predicates, state space, operators, and selection criteria.

The efficiency of problem solving depends on the underlying representations in a person’s mind, which usually also involves personal aspects. Re-analyzing the problem along different dimensions, or changing from one representation to another, can result in arriving at a new understanding of a problem. This is called restructuring . The following example illustrates this:

Two boys of different ages are playing badminton. The older one is a more skilled player, and therefore the outcome of matches between the two becomes predictable. After repeated defeats the younger boy finally loses interest in playing. The older boy now faces a problem, namely that he has no one to play with anymore. The usual options, according to M. Wertheimer (1945/82), range from “offering candy” and “playing a different game” to “not playing at full ability” and “shaming the younger boy into playing.” All of these strategies aim at making the younger boy stay.

The older boy instead comes up with a different solution: He proposes that they should try to keep the birdie in play as long as possible. Thus, they change from a game of competition to one of cooperation. The proposal is happily accepted, and the game is on again. The key in this story is that the older boy restructured the problem, having found that his attitude toward the game made it difficult to keep the younger boy playing. With the new type of game the problem is solved: the older boy is not bored, and the younger boy is not frustrated. In some cases, new representations can make a problem more difficult or much easier to solve. In the latter case insight – the sudden realization of a problem’s solution – may be the key to finding a solution.

There are two very different ways of approaching a goal-oriented situation . In one case an organism readily reproduces the response to the given problem from past experience. This is called reproductive thinking .

The second way requires something new and di fferent to achieve the goal—prior learning is of little help here. Such productive thinking is sometimes argued to involve insight . Gestalt psychologists state that insight problems are a separate category of problems in their own right.

Tasks that might involve insight usually have certain features: they require something new and non-obvious to be done, and in most cases they are difficult enough to predict that the initial solution attempt will be unsuccessful. When you solve a problem of this kind you often have a so called “aha” experience: the solution pops into mind all of a sudden. In one moment you have no idea how to answer the problem, and you feel you are not making any progress trying out different ideas, but in the next moment the problem is solved.

For readers who would like to experience such an effect, here is an example of an insight problem: Knut is given four pieces of a chain; each made up of three links. The task is to link it all up to a closed loop. To open a link costs 2 cents, and to close a link costs 3 cents. Knut has 15 cents to spend. What should Knut do?

Four groups of rings separated from eachother

If you want to know the correct solution, turn to the next page.

To show that solving insight problems involves restructuring , psychologists have created a number of problems that are more difficult to solve for participants with previous experiences, since it is harder for them to change the representation of the given situation.

For non-insight problems the opposite is the case. Solving arithmetical problems, for instance, requires schemas, through which one can get to the solution step by step.

Sometimes, previous experience or familiarity can even make problem solving more difficult. This is the case whenever habitual directions get in the way of finding new directions – an effect called fixation .

FUNCTIONAL FIXEDNESS

Functional fixedness concerns the solution of object use problems . The basic idea is that when the usual function an object is emphasized, it will be far more difficult for a person to use that object in a novel manner. An example for this effect is the candle problem : Imagine you are given a box of matches, some candles and tacks. On the wall of the room there is a cork-board. Your task is to fix the candle to the cork-board in such a way that no wax will drop on the floor when the candle is lit. Got an idea?

Dunker candle problem with matches, candles, and tacs.

Here’s a clue: when people are confronted with a problem and given certain objects to solve it, it is difficult for them to figure out that they could use the objects in a different way. In this example, the box has to be recognized as a support rather than as a container— tack the matchbox to the wall, and place the candle upright in the box. The box will catch the falling wax.

Four groups of rings linked together

A further example is the two-string problem : Knut is left in a room with a pair of pliers and given the task to bind two strings together that are hanging from the ceiling. The problem he faces is that he can never reach both strings at a time because they are just too far away from each other. What can Knut do?

Person holding string reaching for another string

Solution: Knut has to recognize he can use the pliers in a novel function: as weight for a pendulum. He can tie them to one of the strings, push it away, hold the other string and wait for the first one to swing toward him.

MENTAL FIXEDNESS

Functional fixedness as involved in the examples above illustrates a mental set: a person’s tendency to respond to a given task in a manner based on past experience. Because Knut maps an object to a particular function he has difficulty varying the way of use (i.e., pliers as pendulum’s weight).

One approach to studying fixation was to study wrong-answer verbal insight problems . In these probems, people tend to give an incorrect answer when failing to solve a problem rather than to give no answer at all.

A typical example: People are told that on a lake the area covered by water lilies doubles every 24 hours and that it takes 60 days to cover the whole lake. Then they are asked how many days it takes to cover half the lake. The typical response is “30 days” (whereas 59 days is correct).

These wrong solutions are due to an inaccurate interpretation , or representation , of the problem. This can happen because of sloppiness (a quick shallow reading of the problem and/or weak monitoring of their efforts made to come to a solution). In this case error feedback should help people to reconsider the problem features, note the inadequacy of their first answer, and find the correct solution. If, however, people are truly fixated on their incorrect representation, being told the answer is wrong does not help. In a study by P.I. Dallop and

R.L. Dominowski in 1992 these two possibilities were investigated. In approximately one third of the cases error feedback led to right answers, so only approximately one third of the wrong answers were due to inadequate monitoring.

Another approach is the study of examples with and without a preceding analogous task. In cases such like the water-jug task, analogous thinking indeed leads to a correct solution, but to take a different way might make the case much simpler:

Imagine Knut again, this time he is given three jugs with different capacities and is asked to measure the required amount of water. He is not allowed to use anything except the jugs and as much water as he likes. In the first case the sizes are: 127 cups, 21 cups and 3 cups. His goal is to measure 100 cups of water.

In the second case Knut is asked to measure 18 cups from jugs of 39, 15 and 3 cups capacity.

Participants who are given the 100 cup task first choose a complicated way to solve the second task. Participants who did not know about that complex task solved the 18 cup case by just adding three cups to 15.

SOLVING PROBLEMS BY ANALOGY

One special kind of restructuring is analogical problem solving. Here, to find a solution to one problem (i.e., the target problem) an analogous solution to another problem (i.e., the base problem) is presented.

An example for this kind of strategy is the radiation problem posed by K. Duncker in 1945:

As a doctor you have to treat a patient with a malignant, inoperable tumor, buried deep inside the body. There exists a special kind of ray which is harmless at a low intensity, but at sufficiently high intensity is able to destroy the tumor. At such high intensity, however, the ray will also destroy the healthy tissue it passes through on the way to the tumor. What can be done to destroy the tumor while preserving the healthy tissue?

When this question was asked to participants in an experiment, most of them couldn’t come up with the appropriate answer to the problem. Then they were told a story that went something like this:

A general wanted to capture his enemy’s fortress. He gathered a large army to launch a full- scale direct attack, but then learned that all the roads leading directly towards the fortress were blocked by landmines. These roadblocks were designed in such a way that it was possible for small groups of the fortress-owner’s men to pass over them safely, but a large group of men would set them off. The general devised the following plan: He divided his troops into several smaller groups and ordered each of them to march down a different road, timed in such a way that the entire army would reunite exactly when reaching the fortress and could hit with full strength.

Here, the story about the general is the source problem, and the radiation problem is the target problem. The fortress is analogous to the tumor and the big army corresponds to the highly intensive ray. Likewise, a small group of soldiers represents a ray at low intensity. The s olution to the problem is to split the ray up, as the general did with his army, and send the now harmless rays towards the tumor from different angles in such a way that they all meet when reaching it. No healthy tissue is damaged but the tumor itself gets destroyed by the ray at its full intensity.

M. Gick and K. Holyoak presented Duncker’s radiation problem to a group of participants in 1980 and 1983. 10 percent of participants were able to solve the problem right away, but 30 percent could solve it when they read the story of the general before. After being given an additional hint — to use the story as help — 75 percent of them solved the problem.

Following these results, Gick and Holyoak concluded that analogical problem solving consists of three steps:

1.  Recognizing that an analogical connection exists between the source and the base problem.

2. Mapping corresponding parts of the two problems onto each other (fortress ® tumour, army ® ray, etc.)

3. Applying the mapping to generate a parallel solution to the target problem (using little groups of soldiers approaching from different directions ® sending several weaker rays from different directions)

Next, Gick and Holyoak started looking for factors that could help the recognizing and mapping processes.

The abstract concept that links the target problem with the base problem is called the problem schema. Gick and Holyoak facilitated the activation of a schema with their participants by giving them two stories and asking them to compare and summarize them. This activation of problem schemas is called “schema induction“.

The experimenters had participants read stories that presented problems and their solutions. One story was the above story about the general, and other stories required the same problem schema (i.e., if a heavy force coming from one direction is not suitable, use multiple smaller forces that simultaneously converge on the target). The experimenters manipulated how many of these stories the participants read before the participants were asked to solve the radiation problem. The experiment showed that in order to solve the target problem, reading two stories with analogical problems is more helpful than reading only one story. This evidence suggests that schema induction can be achieved by exposing people to multiple problems with the same problem schema.

HOW DO EXPERTS SOLVE PROBLEMS?

An expert is someone who devotes large amounts of their time and energy to one specific field of interest in which they, subsequently, reach a certain level of mastery. It should not be a surprise that experts tend to be better at solving problems in their field than novices (i.e., people who are beginners or not as well-trained in a field as experts) are. Experts are faster at coming up with solutions and have a higher rate of correct solutions. But what is the difference between the way experts and non-experts solve problems? Research on the nature of expertise has come up with the following conclusions:

1.       Experts know more about their field,

2.      their knowledge is organized differently, and

3.      they spend more time analyzing the problem.

Expertise is domain specific— when it comes to problems that are outside the experts’ domain of expertise, their performance often does not differ from that of novices.

Knowledge: An experiment by Chase and Simon (1973) dealt with the question of how well experts and novices are able to reproduce positions of chess pieces on chess boards after a brief presentation. The results showed that experts were far better at reproducing actual game positions, but that their performance was comparable with that of novices when the chess pieces were arranged randomly on the board. Chase and Simon concluded that the superior performance on actual game positions was due to the ability to recognize familiar patterns: A chess expert has up to 50,000 patterns stored in his memory. In comparison, a good player might know about 1,000 patterns by heart and a novice only few to none at all. This very detailed knowledge is of crucial help when an expert is confronted with a new problem in his field. Still, it is not only the amount of knowledge that makes an expert more successful. Experts also organize their knowledge differently from novices.

Organization: In 1981 M. Chi and her co-workers took a set of 24 physics problems and presented them to a group of physics professors as well as to a group of students with only one semester of physics. The task was to group the problems based on their similarities. The students tended to group the problems based on their surface structure (i.e., similarities of objects used in the problem, such as sketches illustrating the problem), whereas the professors used their deep structure (i.e., the general physical principles that underlie the problems) as criteria. By recognizing the actual structure of a problem experts are able to connect the given task to the relevant knowledge they already have (e.g., another problem they solved earlier which required the same strategy).

Analysis: Experts often spend more time analyzing a problem before actually trying to solve it. This way of approaching a problem may often result in what appears to be a slow start, but in the long run this strategy is much more effective. A novice, on the other hand, might start working on the problem right away, but often reach dead ends as they chose a wrong path in the very beginning.

_________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive psychology, 4(1), 55-81.

Chi, M. T., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive science, 5(2), 121-152.

Duncker, K., & Lees, L. S. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological monographs, 58(5).

Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive psychology, 12(3), 306-355. Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive psychology, 15(1), 1-38.

Goldstein, E.B. (2005). Cogntive Psychology. Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience. Belmont: Thomson Wadsworth.

R.L. Dominowski and P. Dallob, Insight and Problem Solving. In The Nature of Insight, R.J. Sternberg & J.E. Davidson (Eds). MIT Press: USA, pp.33-62 (1995).

Wertheimer, M., (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.

ESSENTIALS OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Copyright © 2023 by Christopher Klein is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

IMAGES

  1. SOLUTION: FACTORS AFFECTING PROBLEM SOLVING , IMPROVING PROBLEM SOLVING

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  2. SOLUTION: FACTORS AFFECTING PROBLEM SOLVING , IMPROVING PROBLEM SOLVING

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  3. Factors contributing to successful problem solving.

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  4. Factors affecting Problem Solving #psychology #ignou #psychologist #mapc #mapsychology

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  5. Psychology, topic factors affecting problem solving

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  6. A-AS Level (CIE) Psychology Specimen Questions with Answers 105 to 106

    what are the factors affecting problem solving in psychology

VIDEO

  1. Factors affecting Problem Solving #psychology #ignou #psychologist #mapc #mapsychology

  2. Analogy Tips and Tricks/Reasoning questions

  3. Creativity and Mood #creativity #mood #psychology

  4. Psychology: Thinking and Problem Solving

  5. Cognitive Psychology (Class #20)

  6. Don't Fall for THESE Mental Traps 😮 #criticalthinking

COMMENTS

  1. Problem-Solving Strategies and Obstacles

    Problem-solving is not a flawless process as there are a number of obstacles that can interfere with our ability to solve a problem quickly and efficiently. These obstacles include: Assumptions: When dealing with a problem, people can make assumptions about the constraints and obstacles that prevent certain solutions.

  2. PDF The Psychology of Problem Solving

    The Psychology of Problem Solving Problems are a central part of human life. The Psychology of Problem Solving organizes in one volume much of what psychologists know about problem solving and the factors that contribute to its success or failure. There are chapters by leading experts in this field, includ-

  3. What Affects Problem Solving

    Acknowledging the factors that affect the process and taking them into account when forming a solution gives teams the best chance of solving the problem effectively. Below we have outlined the key factors affecting the problem solving process. Understanding the problem The most important factor in solving a problem is to first fully understand it.

  4. The Psychology of Problem Solving

    The Psychology of Problem Solving organizes in one volume much of what psychologists know about problem solving and the factors that contribute to its success or failure. There are chapters by leading experts in this field, including Miriam Bassok, Randall Engle, Anders Ericsson, Arthur Graesser, Keith Stanovich, Norbert Schwarz, and Barry ...

  5. Facilitating and Hindering Factors in Problem Solving

    A person may not have all the needed resources, which can hinder solving a problem. Facilitating Factors. Though many factors hinder problem-solving, on the other hand, there are facilitating factors as well: Motivation − motivation is positively correlated with problem-solving skills. This means that the higher the motivation, the more ...

  6. Identifying Barriers to Problem-Solving in Psychology

    Problem-solving in psychology refers to the cognitive processes through which individuals identify and overcome obstacles or challenges to reach a desired goal, drawing on various mental processes and strategies. ... By being aware of their own emotions and biases, individuals can better understand how these factors may be affecting their ...

  7. On the cognitive process of human problem solving

    This section surveys related studies on problem solving and its cognitive foundations in psychology and computational intelligence. The approaches to and factors affecting problem solving are explored. The cognitive characteristics of problem solvers and the impact of their knowledge in problem solving are discussed.

  8. Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective

    Background The Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development emphasizes the importance of complex problem-solving (CPS) skills in the 21st century. CPS skills have been linked to academic performance, career development, and job competency training. Reflective learning, which includes journal writing, peer reflection, selfreflection, and group discussion, has been explored to improve ...

  9. PDF The Psychology of Problem Solving

    The psychology of problem solving / edited by Janet E. Davidson, Robert J. Sternberg. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. isbn -521-79333-5 - isbn -521-79741-1 (pb.) ... problem-solving process while it is ongoing, and (8) evaluating the solu-tion to the problem after problem solving is completed. In this theoretical

  10. Chapter 9. Problem-Solving

    One dominant approach to problem solving originated from Gestalt psychologists in the 1920s. Their understanding of problem solving emphasizes behavior in situations requiring relatively novel means of attaining goals and suggests that problem solving involves a process called restructuring.