Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

A research hypothesis, in its plural form “hypotheses,” is a specific, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its outset. It is a key component of the scientific method .

Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding

Some key points about hypotheses:

  • A hypothesis expresses an expected pattern or relationship. It connects the variables under investigation.
  • It is stated in clear, precise terms before any data collection or analysis occurs. This makes the hypothesis testable.
  • A hypothesis must be falsifiable. It should be possible, even if unlikely in practice, to collect data that disconfirms rather than supports the hypothesis.
  • Hypotheses guide research. Scientists design studies to explicitly evaluate hypotheses about how nature works.
  • For a hypothesis to be valid, it must be testable against empirical evidence. The evidence can then confirm or disprove the testable predictions.
  • Hypotheses are informed by background knowledge and observation, but go beyond what is already known to propose an explanation of how or why something occurs.
Predictions typically arise from a thorough knowledge of the research literature, curiosity about real-world problems or implications, and integrating this to advance theory. They build on existing literature while providing new insight.

Types of Research Hypotheses

Alternative hypothesis.

The research hypothesis is often called the alternative or experimental hypothesis in experimental research.

It typically suggests a potential relationship between two key variables: the independent variable, which the researcher manipulates, and the dependent variable, which is measured based on those changes.

The alternative hypothesis states a relationship exists between the two variables being studied (one variable affects the other).

A hypothesis is a testable statement or prediction about the relationship between two or more variables. It is a key component of the scientific method. Some key points about hypotheses:

  • Important hypotheses lead to predictions that can be tested empirically. The evidence can then confirm or disprove the testable predictions.

In summary, a hypothesis is a precise, testable statement of what researchers expect to happen in a study and why. Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding.

An experimental hypothesis predicts what change(s) will occur in the dependent variable when the independent variable is manipulated.

It states that the results are not due to chance and are significant in supporting the theory being investigated.

The alternative hypothesis can be directional, indicating a specific direction of the effect, or non-directional, suggesting a difference without specifying its nature. It’s what researchers aim to support or demonstrate through their study.

Null Hypothesis

The null hypothesis states no relationship exists between the two variables being studied (one variable does not affect the other). There will be no changes in the dependent variable due to manipulating the independent variable.

It states results are due to chance and are not significant in supporting the idea being investigated.

The null hypothesis, positing no effect or relationship, is a foundational contrast to the research hypothesis in scientific inquiry. It establishes a baseline for statistical testing, promoting objectivity by initiating research from a neutral stance.

Many statistical methods are tailored to test the null hypothesis, determining the likelihood of observed results if no true effect exists.

This dual-hypothesis approach provides clarity, ensuring that research intentions are explicit, and fosters consistency across scientific studies, enhancing the standardization and interpretability of research outcomes.

Nondirectional Hypothesis

A non-directional hypothesis, also known as a two-tailed hypothesis, predicts that there is a difference or relationship between two variables but does not specify the direction of this relationship.

It merely indicates that a change or effect will occur without predicting which group will have higher or lower values.

For example, “There is a difference in performance between Group A and Group B” is a non-directional hypothesis.

Directional Hypothesis

A directional (one-tailed) hypothesis predicts the nature of the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable. It predicts in which direction the change will take place. (i.e., greater, smaller, less, more)

It specifies whether one variable is greater, lesser, or different from another, rather than just indicating that there’s a difference without specifying its nature.

For example, “Exercise increases weight loss” is a directional hypothesis.

hypothesis

Falsifiability

The Falsification Principle, proposed by Karl Popper , is a way of demarcating science from non-science. It suggests that for a theory or hypothesis to be considered scientific, it must be testable and irrefutable.

Falsifiability emphasizes that scientific claims shouldn’t just be confirmable but should also have the potential to be proven wrong.

It means that there should exist some potential evidence or experiment that could prove the proposition false.

However many confirming instances exist for a theory, it only takes one counter observation to falsify it. For example, the hypothesis that “all swans are white,” can be falsified by observing a black swan.

For Popper, science should attempt to disprove a theory rather than attempt to continually provide evidence to support a research hypothesis.

Can a Hypothesis be Proven?

Hypotheses make probabilistic predictions. They state the expected outcome if a particular relationship exists. However, a study result supporting a hypothesis does not definitively prove it is true.

All studies have limitations. There may be unknown confounding factors or issues that limit the certainty of conclusions. Additional studies may yield different results.

In science, hypotheses can realistically only be supported with some degree of confidence, not proven. The process of science is to incrementally accumulate evidence for and against hypothesized relationships in an ongoing pursuit of better models and explanations that best fit the empirical data. But hypotheses remain open to revision and rejection if that is where the evidence leads.
  • Disproving a hypothesis is definitive. Solid disconfirmatory evidence will falsify a hypothesis and require altering or discarding it based on the evidence.
  • However, confirming evidence is always open to revision. Other explanations may account for the same results, and additional or contradictory evidence may emerge over time.

We can never 100% prove the alternative hypothesis. Instead, we see if we can disprove, or reject the null hypothesis.

If we reject the null hypothesis, this doesn’t mean that our alternative hypothesis is correct but does support the alternative/experimental hypothesis.

Upon analysis of the results, an alternative hypothesis can be rejected or supported, but it can never be proven to be correct. We must avoid any reference to results proving a theory as this implies 100% certainty, and there is always a chance that evidence may exist which could refute a theory.

How to Write a Hypothesis

  • Identify variables . The researcher manipulates the independent variable and the dependent variable is the measured outcome.
  • Operationalized the variables being investigated . Operationalization of a hypothesis refers to the process of making the variables physically measurable or testable, e.g. if you are about to study aggression, you might count the number of punches given by participants.
  • Decide on a direction for your prediction . If there is evidence in the literature to support a specific effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, write a directional (one-tailed) hypothesis. If there are limited or ambiguous findings in the literature regarding the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, write a non-directional (two-tailed) hypothesis.
  • Make it Testable : Ensure your hypothesis can be tested through experimentation or observation. It should be possible to prove it false (principle of falsifiability).
  • Clear & concise language . A strong hypothesis is concise (typically one to two sentences long), and formulated using clear and straightforward language, ensuring it’s easily understood and testable.

Consider a hypothesis many teachers might subscribe to: students work better on Monday morning than on Friday afternoon (IV=Day, DV= Standard of work).

Now, if we decide to study this by giving the same group of students a lesson on a Monday morning and a Friday afternoon and then measuring their immediate recall of the material covered in each session, we would end up with the following:

  • The alternative hypothesis states that students will recall significantly more information on a Monday morning than on a Friday afternoon.
  • The null hypothesis states that there will be no significant difference in the amount recalled on a Monday morning compared to a Friday afternoon. Any difference will be due to chance or confounding factors.

More Examples

  • Memory : Participants exposed to classical music during study sessions will recall more items from a list than those who studied in silence.
  • Social Psychology : Individuals who frequently engage in social media use will report higher levels of perceived social isolation compared to those who use it infrequently.
  • Developmental Psychology : Children who engage in regular imaginative play have better problem-solving skills than those who don’t.
  • Clinical Psychology : Cognitive-behavioral therapy will be more effective in reducing symptoms of anxiety over a 6-month period compared to traditional talk therapy.
  • Cognitive Psychology : Individuals who multitask between various electronic devices will have shorter attention spans on focused tasks than those who single-task.
  • Health Psychology : Patients who practice mindfulness meditation will experience lower levels of chronic pain compared to those who don’t meditate.
  • Organizational Psychology : Employees in open-plan offices will report higher levels of stress than those in private offices.
  • Behavioral Psychology : Rats rewarded with food after pressing a lever will press it more frequently than rats who receive no reward.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

How to Write a Great Hypothesis

Hypothesis Definition, Format, Examples, and Tips

Verywell / Alex Dos Diaz

  • The Scientific Method

Hypothesis Format

Falsifiability of a hypothesis.

  • Operationalization

Hypothesis Types

Hypotheses examples.

  • Collecting Data

A hypothesis is a tentative statement about the relationship between two or more variables. It is a specific, testable prediction about what you expect to happen in a study. It is a preliminary answer to your question that helps guide the research process.

Consider a study designed to examine the relationship between sleep deprivation and test performance. The hypothesis might be: "This study is designed to assess the hypothesis that sleep-deprived people will perform worse on a test than individuals who are not sleep-deprived."

At a Glance

A hypothesis is crucial to scientific research because it offers a clear direction for what the researchers are looking to find. This allows them to design experiments to test their predictions and add to our scientific knowledge about the world. This article explores how a hypothesis is used in psychology research, how to write a good hypothesis, and the different types of hypotheses you might use.

The Hypothesis in the Scientific Method

In the scientific method , whether it involves research in psychology, biology, or some other area, a hypothesis represents what the researchers think will happen in an experiment. The scientific method involves the following steps:

  • Forming a question
  • Performing background research
  • Creating a hypothesis
  • Designing an experiment
  • Collecting data
  • Analyzing the results
  • Drawing conclusions
  • Communicating the results

The hypothesis is a prediction, but it involves more than a guess. Most of the time, the hypothesis begins with a question which is then explored through background research. At this point, researchers then begin to develop a testable hypothesis.

Unless you are creating an exploratory study, your hypothesis should always explain what you  expect  to happen.

In a study exploring the effects of a particular drug, the hypothesis might be that researchers expect the drug to have some type of effect on the symptoms of a specific illness. In psychology, the hypothesis might focus on how a certain aspect of the environment might influence a particular behavior.

Remember, a hypothesis does not have to be correct. While the hypothesis predicts what the researchers expect to see, the goal of the research is to determine whether this guess is right or wrong. When conducting an experiment, researchers might explore numerous factors to determine which ones might contribute to the ultimate outcome.

In many cases, researchers may find that the results of an experiment  do not  support the original hypothesis. When writing up these results, the researchers might suggest other options that should be explored in future studies.

In many cases, researchers might draw a hypothesis from a specific theory or build on previous research. For example, prior research has shown that stress can impact the immune system. So a researcher might hypothesize: "People with high-stress levels will be more likely to contract a common cold after being exposed to the virus than people who have low-stress levels."

In other instances, researchers might look at commonly held beliefs or folk wisdom. "Birds of a feather flock together" is one example of folk adage that a psychologist might try to investigate. The researcher might pose a specific hypothesis that "People tend to select romantic partners who are similar to them in interests and educational level."

Elements of a Good Hypothesis

So how do you write a good hypothesis? When trying to come up with a hypothesis for your research or experiments, ask yourself the following questions:

  • Is your hypothesis based on your research on a topic?
  • Can your hypothesis be tested?
  • Does your hypothesis include independent and dependent variables?

Before you come up with a specific hypothesis, spend some time doing background research. Once you have completed a literature review, start thinking about potential questions you still have. Pay attention to the discussion section in the  journal articles you read . Many authors will suggest questions that still need to be explored.

How to Formulate a Good Hypothesis

To form a hypothesis, you should take these steps:

  • Collect as many observations about a topic or problem as you can.
  • Evaluate these observations and look for possible causes of the problem.
  • Create a list of possible explanations that you might want to explore.
  • After you have developed some possible hypotheses, think of ways that you could confirm or disprove each hypothesis through experimentation. This is known as falsifiability.

In the scientific method ,  falsifiability is an important part of any valid hypothesis. In order to test a claim scientifically, it must be possible that the claim could be proven false.

Students sometimes confuse the idea of falsifiability with the idea that it means that something is false, which is not the case. What falsifiability means is that  if  something was false, then it is possible to demonstrate that it is false.

One of the hallmarks of pseudoscience is that it makes claims that cannot be refuted or proven false.

The Importance of Operational Definitions

A variable is a factor or element that can be changed and manipulated in ways that are observable and measurable. However, the researcher must also define how the variable will be manipulated and measured in the study.

Operational definitions are specific definitions for all relevant factors in a study. This process helps make vague or ambiguous concepts detailed and measurable.

For example, a researcher might operationally define the variable " test anxiety " as the results of a self-report measure of anxiety experienced during an exam. A "study habits" variable might be defined by the amount of studying that actually occurs as measured by time.

These precise descriptions are important because many things can be measured in various ways. Clearly defining these variables and how they are measured helps ensure that other researchers can replicate your results.

Replicability

One of the basic principles of any type of scientific research is that the results must be replicable.

Replication means repeating an experiment in the same way to produce the same results. By clearly detailing the specifics of how the variables were measured and manipulated, other researchers can better understand the results and repeat the study if needed.

Some variables are more difficult than others to define. For example, how would you operationally define a variable such as aggression ? For obvious ethical reasons, researchers cannot create a situation in which a person behaves aggressively toward others.

To measure this variable, the researcher must devise a measurement that assesses aggressive behavior without harming others. The researcher might utilize a simulated task to measure aggressiveness in this situation.

Hypothesis Checklist

  • Does your hypothesis focus on something that you can actually test?
  • Does your hypothesis include both an independent and dependent variable?
  • Can you manipulate the variables?
  • Can your hypothesis be tested without violating ethical standards?

The hypothesis you use will depend on what you are investigating and hoping to find. Some of the main types of hypotheses that you might use include:

  • Simple hypothesis : This type of hypothesis suggests there is a relationship between one independent variable and one dependent variable.
  • Complex hypothesis : This type suggests a relationship between three or more variables, such as two independent and dependent variables.
  • Null hypothesis : This hypothesis suggests no relationship exists between two or more variables.
  • Alternative hypothesis : This hypothesis states the opposite of the null hypothesis.
  • Statistical hypothesis : This hypothesis uses statistical analysis to evaluate a representative population sample and then generalizes the findings to the larger group.
  • Logical hypothesis : This hypothesis assumes a relationship between variables without collecting data or evidence.

A hypothesis often follows a basic format of "If {this happens} then {this will happen}." One way to structure your hypothesis is to describe what will happen to the  dependent variable  if you change the  independent variable .

The basic format might be: "If {these changes are made to a certain independent variable}, then we will observe {a change in a specific dependent variable}."

A few examples of simple hypotheses:

  • "Students who eat breakfast will perform better on a math exam than students who do not eat breakfast."
  • "Students who experience test anxiety before an English exam will get lower scores than students who do not experience test anxiety."​
  • "Motorists who talk on the phone while driving will be more likely to make errors on a driving course than those who do not talk on the phone."
  • "Children who receive a new reading intervention will have higher reading scores than students who do not receive the intervention."

Examples of a complex hypothesis include:

  • "People with high-sugar diets and sedentary activity levels are more likely to develop depression."
  • "Younger people who are regularly exposed to green, outdoor areas have better subjective well-being than older adults who have limited exposure to green spaces."

Examples of a null hypothesis include:

  • "There is no difference in anxiety levels between people who take St. John's wort supplements and those who do not."
  • "There is no difference in scores on a memory recall task between children and adults."
  • "There is no difference in aggression levels between children who play first-person shooter games and those who do not."

Examples of an alternative hypothesis:

  • "People who take St. John's wort supplements will have less anxiety than those who do not."
  • "Adults will perform better on a memory task than children."
  • "Children who play first-person shooter games will show higher levels of aggression than children who do not." 

Collecting Data on Your Hypothesis

Once a researcher has formed a testable hypothesis, the next step is to select a research design and start collecting data. The research method depends largely on exactly what they are studying. There are two basic types of research methods: descriptive research and experimental research.

Descriptive Research Methods

Descriptive research such as  case studies ,  naturalistic observations , and surveys are often used when  conducting an experiment is difficult or impossible. These methods are best used to describe different aspects of a behavior or psychological phenomenon.

Once a researcher has collected data using descriptive methods, a  correlational study  can examine how the variables are related. This research method might be used to investigate a hypothesis that is difficult to test experimentally.

Experimental Research Methods

Experimental methods  are used to demonstrate causal relationships between variables. In an experiment, the researcher systematically manipulates a variable of interest (known as the independent variable) and measures the effect on another variable (known as the dependent variable).

Unlike correlational studies, which can only be used to determine if there is a relationship between two variables, experimental methods can be used to determine the actual nature of the relationship—whether changes in one variable actually  cause  another to change.

The hypothesis is a critical part of any scientific exploration. It represents what researchers expect to find in a study or experiment. In situations where the hypothesis is unsupported by the research, the research still has value. Such research helps us better understand how different aspects of the natural world relate to one another. It also helps us develop new hypotheses that can then be tested in the future.

Thompson WH, Skau S. On the scope of scientific hypotheses .  R Soc Open Sci . 2023;10(8):230607. doi:10.1098/rsos.230607

Taran S, Adhikari NKJ, Fan E. Falsifiability in medicine: what clinicians can learn from Karl Popper [published correction appears in Intensive Care Med. 2021 Jun 17;:].  Intensive Care Med . 2021;47(9):1054-1056. doi:10.1007/s00134-021-06432-z

Eyler AA. Research Methods for Public Health . 1st ed. Springer Publishing Company; 2020. doi:10.1891/9780826182067.0004

Nosek BA, Errington TM. What is replication ?  PLoS Biol . 2020;18(3):e3000691. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000691

Aggarwal R, Ranganathan P. Study designs: Part 2 - Descriptive studies .  Perspect Clin Res . 2019;10(1):34-36. doi:10.4103/picr.PICR_154_18

Nevid J. Psychology: Concepts and Applications. Wadworth, 2013.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • Games & Quizzes
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center

experiments disproving spontaneous generation

  • When did science begin?
  • Where was science invented?

Blackboard inscribed with scientific formulas and calculations in physics and mathematics

scientific hypothesis

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • National Center for Biotechnology Information - PubMed Central - On the scope of scientific hypotheses
  • LiveScience - What is a scientific hypothesis?
  • The Royal Society - Open Science - On the scope of scientific hypotheses

experiments disproving spontaneous generation

scientific hypothesis , an idea that proposes a tentative explanation about a phenomenon or a narrow set of phenomena observed in the natural world. The two primary features of a scientific hypothesis are falsifiability and testability, which are reflected in an “If…then” statement summarizing the idea and in the ability to be supported or refuted through observation and experimentation. The notion of the scientific hypothesis as both falsifiable and testable was advanced in the mid-20th century by Austrian-born British philosopher Karl Popper .

The formulation and testing of a hypothesis is part of the scientific method , the approach scientists use when attempting to understand and test ideas about natural phenomena. The generation of a hypothesis frequently is described as a creative process and is based on existing scientific knowledge, intuition , or experience. Therefore, although scientific hypotheses commonly are described as educated guesses, they actually are more informed than a guess. In addition, scientists generally strive to develop simple hypotheses, since these are easier to test relative to hypotheses that involve many different variables and potential outcomes. Such complex hypotheses may be developed as scientific models ( see scientific modeling ).

Depending on the results of scientific evaluation, a hypothesis typically is either rejected as false or accepted as true. However, because a hypothesis inherently is falsifiable, even hypotheses supported by scientific evidence and accepted as true are susceptible to rejection later, when new evidence has become available. In some instances, rather than rejecting a hypothesis because it has been falsified by new evidence, scientists simply adapt the existing idea to accommodate the new information. In this sense a hypothesis is never incorrect but only incomplete.

The investigation of scientific hypotheses is an important component in the development of scientific theory . Hence, hypotheses differ fundamentally from theories; whereas the former is a specific tentative explanation and serves as the main tool by which scientists gather data, the latter is a broad general explanation that incorporates data from many different scientific investigations undertaken to explore hypotheses.

Countless hypotheses have been developed and tested throughout the history of science . Several examples include the idea that living organisms develop from nonliving matter, which formed the basis of spontaneous generation , a hypothesis that ultimately was disproved (first in 1668, with the experiments of Italian physician Francesco Redi , and later in 1859, with the experiments of French chemist and microbiologist Louis Pasteur ); the concept proposed in the late 19th century that microorganisms cause certain diseases (now known as germ theory ); and the notion that oceanic crust forms along submarine mountain zones and spreads laterally away from them ( seafloor spreading hypothesis ).

2.4 Developing a Hypothesis

Learning objectives.

  • Distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis.
  • Discover how theories are used to generate hypotheses and how the results of studies can be used to further inform theories.
  • Understand the characteristics of a good hypothesis.

Theories and Hypotheses

Before describing how to develop a hypothesis it is imporant to distinguish betwee a theory and a hypothesis. A  theory  is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions, or organizing principles that have not been observed directly. Consider, for example, Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation and social inhibition. He proposed that being watched by others while performing a task creates a general state of physiological arousal, which increases the likelihood of the dominant (most likely) response. So for highly practiced tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make correct responses, but for relatively unpracticed tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make incorrect responses. Notice that this theory—which has come to be called drive theory—provides an explanation of both social facilitation and social inhibition that goes beyond the phenomena themselves by including concepts such as “arousal” and “dominant response,” along with processes such as the effect of arousal on the dominant response.

Outside of science, referring to an idea as a theory often implies that it is untested—perhaps no more than a wild guess. In science, however, the term theory has no such implication. A theory is simply an explanation or interpretation of a set of phenomena. It can be untested, but it can also be extensively tested, well supported, and accepted as an accurate description of the world by the scientific community. The theory of evolution by natural selection, for example, is a theory because it is an explanation of the diversity of life on earth—not because it is untested or unsupported by scientific research. On the contrary, the evidence for this theory is overwhelmingly positive and nearly all scientists accept its basic assumptions as accurate. Similarly, the “germ theory” of disease is a theory because it is an explanation of the origin of various diseases, not because there is any doubt that many diseases are caused by microorganisms that infect the body.

A  hypothesis , on the other hand, is a specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate. It is an explanation that relies on just a few key concepts. Hypotheses are often specific predictions about what will happen in a particular study. They are developed by considering existing evidence and using reasoning to infer what will happen in the specific context of interest. Hypotheses are often but not always derived from theories. So a hypothesis is often a prediction based on a theory but some hypotheses are a-theoretical and only after a set of observations have been made, is a theory developed. This is because theories are broad in nature and they explain larger bodies of data. So if our research question is really original then we may need to collect some data and make some observation before we can develop a broader theory.

Theories and hypotheses always have this  if-then  relationship. “ If   drive theory is correct,  then  cockroaches should run through a straight runway faster, and a branching runway more slowly, when other cockroaches are present.” Although hypotheses are usually expressed as statements, they can always be rephrased as questions. “Do cockroaches run through a straight runway faster when other cockroaches are present?” Thus deriving hypotheses from theories is an excellent way of generating interesting research questions.

But how do researchers derive hypotheses from theories? One way is to generate a research question using the techniques discussed in this chapter  and then ask whether any theory implies an answer to that question. For example, you might wonder whether expressive writing about positive experiences improves health as much as expressive writing about traumatic experiences. Although this  question  is an interesting one  on its own, you might then ask whether the habituation theory—the idea that expressive writing causes people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings—implies an answer. In this case, it seems clear that if the habituation theory is correct, then expressive writing about positive experiences should not be effective because it would not cause people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings. A second way to derive hypotheses from theories is to focus on some component of the theory that has not yet been directly observed. For example, a researcher could focus on the process of habituation—perhaps hypothesizing that people should show fewer signs of emotional distress with each new writing session.

Among the very best hypotheses are those that distinguish between competing theories. For example, Norbert Schwarz and his colleagues considered two theories of how people make judgments about themselves, such as how assertive they are (Schwarz et al., 1991) [1] . Both theories held that such judgments are based on relevant examples that people bring to mind. However, one theory was that people base their judgments on the  number  of examples they bring to mind and the other was that people base their judgments on how  easily  they bring those examples to mind. To test these theories, the researchers asked people to recall either six times when they were assertive (which is easy for most people) or 12 times (which is difficult for most people). Then they asked them to judge their own assertiveness. Note that the number-of-examples theory implies that people who recalled 12 examples should judge themselves to be more assertive because they recalled more examples, but the ease-of-examples theory implies that participants who recalled six examples should judge themselves as more assertive because recalling the examples was easier. Thus the two theories made opposite predictions so that only one of the predictions could be confirmed. The surprising result was that participants who recalled fewer examples judged themselves to be more assertive—providing particularly convincing evidence in favor of the ease-of-retrieval theory over the number-of-examples theory.

Theory Testing

The primary way that scientific researchers use theories is sometimes called the hypothetico-deductive method  (although this term is much more likely to be used by philosophers of science than by scientists themselves). A researcher begins with a set of phenomena and either constructs a theory to explain or interpret them or chooses an existing theory to work with. He or she then makes a prediction about some new phenomenon that should be observed if the theory is correct. Again, this prediction is called a hypothesis. The researcher then conducts an empirical study to test the hypothesis. Finally, he or she reevaluates the theory in light of the new results and revises it if necessary. This process is usually conceptualized as a cycle because the researcher can then derive a new hypothesis from the revised theory, conduct a new empirical study to test the hypothesis, and so on. As  Figure 2.2  shows, this approach meshes nicely with the model of scientific research in psychology presented earlier in the textbook—creating a more detailed model of “theoretically motivated” or “theory-driven” research.

Figure 4.4 Hypothetico-Deductive Method Combined With the General Model of Scientific Research in Psychology Together they form a model of theoretically motivated research.

Figure 2.2 Hypothetico-Deductive Method Combined With the General Model of Scientific Research in Psychology Together they form a model of theoretically motivated research.

As an example, let us consider Zajonc’s research on social facilitation and inhibition. He started with a somewhat contradictory pattern of results from the research literature. He then constructed his drive theory, according to which being watched by others while performing a task causes physiological arousal, which increases an organism’s tendency to make the dominant response. This theory predicts social facilitation for well-learned tasks and social inhibition for poorly learned tasks. He now had a theory that organized previous results in a meaningful way—but he still needed to test it. He hypothesized that if his theory was correct, he should observe that the presence of others improves performance in a simple laboratory task but inhibits performance in a difficult version of the very same laboratory task. To test this hypothesis, one of the studies he conducted used cockroaches as subjects (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969) [2] . The cockroaches ran either down a straight runway (an easy task for a cockroach) or through a cross-shaped maze (a difficult task for a cockroach) to escape into a dark chamber when a light was shined on them. They did this either while alone or in the presence of other cockroaches in clear plastic “audience boxes.” Zajonc found that cockroaches in the straight runway reached their goal more quickly in the presence of other cockroaches, but cockroaches in the cross-shaped maze reached their goal more slowly when they were in the presence of other cockroaches. Thus he confirmed his hypothesis and provided support for his drive theory. (Zajonc also showed that drive theory existed in humans (Zajonc & Sales, 1966) [3] in many other studies afterward).

Incorporating Theory into Your Research

When you write your research report or plan your presentation, be aware that there are two basic ways that researchers usually include theory. The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more existing theories, derive a hypothesis from one of those theories, test the hypothesis in a new study, and finally reevaluate the theory. This format works well when there is an existing theory that addresses the research question—especially if the resulting hypothesis is surprising or conflicts with a hypothesis derived from a different theory.

To use theories in your research will not only give you guidance in coming up with experiment ideas and possible projects, but it lends legitimacy to your work. Psychologists have been interested in a variety of human behaviors and have developed many theories along the way. Using established theories will help you break new ground as a researcher, not limit you from developing your own ideas.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

There are three general characteristics of a good hypothesis. First, a good hypothesis must be testable and falsifiable . We must be able to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and if you’ll recall Popper’s falsifiability criterion, it must be possible to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false. Second, a good hypothesis must be  logical. As described above, hypotheses are more than just a random guess. Hypotheses should be informed by previous theories or observations and logical reasoning. Typically, we begin with a broad and general theory and use  deductive reasoning to generate a more specific hypothesis to test based on that theory. Occasionally, however, when there is no theory to inform our hypothesis, we use  inductive reasoning  which involves using specific observations or research findings to form a more general hypothesis. Finally, the hypothesis should be  positive.  That is, the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a relationship or effect, rather than a statement that a relationship or effect does not exist. As scientists, we don’t set out to show that relationships do not exist or that effects do not occur so our hypotheses should not be worded in a way to suggest that an effect or relationship does not exist. The nature of science is to assume that something does not exist and then seek to find evidence to prove this wrong, to show that really it does exist. That may seem backward to you but that is the nature of the scientific method. The underlying reason for this is beyond the scope of this chapter but it has to do with statistical theory.

Key Takeaways

  • A theory is broad in nature and explains larger bodies of data. A hypothesis is more specific and makes a prediction about the outcome of a particular study.
  • Working with theories is not “icing on the cake.” It is a basic ingredient of psychological research.
  • Like other scientists, psychologists use the hypothetico-deductive method. They construct theories to explain or interpret phenomena (or work with existing theories), derive hypotheses from their theories, test the hypotheses, and then reevaluate the theories in light of the new results.
  • Practice: Find a recent empirical research report in a professional journal. Read the introduction and highlight in different colors descriptions of theories and hypotheses.
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 195–202. ↵
  • Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. M. (1969). Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13 , 83–92. ↵
  • Zajonc, R.B. & Sales, S.M. (1966). Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 , 160-168. ↵

Creative Commons License

Share This Book

  • Increase Font Size

Educational resources and simple solutions for your research journey

Research hypothesis: What it is, how to write it, types, and examples

What is a Research Hypothesis: How to Write it, Types, and Examples

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

Any research begins with a research question and a research hypothesis . A research question alone may not suffice to design the experiment(s) needed to answer it. A hypothesis is central to the scientific method. But what is a hypothesis ? A hypothesis is a testable statement that proposes a possible explanation to a phenomenon, and it may include a prediction. Next, you may ask what is a research hypothesis ? Simply put, a research hypothesis is a prediction or educated guess about the relationship between the variables that you want to investigate.  

It is important to be thorough when developing your research hypothesis. Shortcomings in the framing of a hypothesis can affect the study design and the results. A better understanding of the research hypothesis definition and characteristics of a good hypothesis will make it easier for you to develop your own hypothesis for your research. Let’s dive in to know more about the types of research hypothesis , how to write a research hypothesis , and some research hypothesis examples .  

Table of Contents

What is a hypothesis ?  

A hypothesis is based on the existing body of knowledge in a study area. Framed before the data are collected, a hypothesis states the tentative relationship between independent and dependent variables, along with a prediction of the outcome.  

What is a research hypothesis ?  

Young researchers starting out their journey are usually brimming with questions like “ What is a hypothesis ?” “ What is a research hypothesis ?” “How can I write a good research hypothesis ?”   

A research hypothesis is a statement that proposes a possible explanation for an observable phenomenon or pattern. It guides the direction of a study and predicts the outcome of the investigation. A research hypothesis is testable, i.e., it can be supported or disproven through experimentation or observation.     

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

Characteristics of a good hypothesis  

Here are the characteristics of a good hypothesis :  

  • Clearly formulated and free of language errors and ambiguity  
  • Concise and not unnecessarily verbose  
  • Has clearly defined variables  
  • Testable and stated in a way that allows for it to be disproven  
  • Can be tested using a research design that is feasible, ethical, and practical   
  • Specific and relevant to the research problem  
  • Rooted in a thorough literature search  
  • Can generate new knowledge or understanding.  

How to create an effective research hypothesis  

A study begins with the formulation of a research question. A researcher then performs background research. This background information forms the basis for building a good research hypothesis . The researcher then performs experiments, collects, and analyzes the data, interprets the findings, and ultimately, determines if the findings support or negate the original hypothesis.  

Let’s look at each step for creating an effective, testable, and good research hypothesis :  

  • Identify a research problem or question: Start by identifying a specific research problem.   
  • Review the literature: Conduct an in-depth review of the existing literature related to the research problem to grasp the current knowledge and gaps in the field.   
  • Formulate a clear and testable hypothesis : Based on the research question, use existing knowledge to form a clear and testable hypothesis . The hypothesis should state a predicted relationship between two or more variables that can be measured and manipulated. Improve the original draft till it is clear and meaningful.  
  • State the null hypothesis: The null hypothesis is a statement that there is no relationship between the variables you are studying.   
  • Define the population and sample: Clearly define the population you are studying and the sample you will be using for your research.  
  • Select appropriate methods for testing the hypothesis: Select appropriate research methods, such as experiments, surveys, or observational studies, which will allow you to test your research hypothesis .  

Remember that creating a research hypothesis is an iterative process, i.e., you might have to revise it based on the data you collect. You may need to test and reject several hypotheses before answering the research problem.  

How to write a research hypothesis  

When you start writing a research hypothesis , you use an “if–then” statement format, which states the predicted relationship between two or more variables. Clearly identify the independent variables (the variables being changed) and the dependent variables (the variables being measured), as well as the population you are studying. Review and revise your hypothesis as needed.  

An example of a research hypothesis in this format is as follows:  

“ If [athletes] follow [cold water showers daily], then their [endurance] increases.”  

Population: athletes  

Independent variable: daily cold water showers  

Dependent variable: endurance  

You may have understood the characteristics of a good hypothesis . But note that a research hypothesis is not always confirmed; a researcher should be prepared to accept or reject the hypothesis based on the study findings.  

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

Research hypothesis checklist  

Following from above, here is a 10-point checklist for a good research hypothesis :  

  • Testable: A research hypothesis should be able to be tested via experimentation or observation.  
  • Specific: A research hypothesis should clearly state the relationship between the variables being studied.  
  • Based on prior research: A research hypothesis should be based on existing knowledge and previous research in the field.  
  • Falsifiable: A research hypothesis should be able to be disproven through testing.  
  • Clear and concise: A research hypothesis should be stated in a clear and concise manner.  
  • Logical: A research hypothesis should be logical and consistent with current understanding of the subject.  
  • Relevant: A research hypothesis should be relevant to the research question and objectives.  
  • Feasible: A research hypothesis should be feasible to test within the scope of the study.  
  • Reflects the population: A research hypothesis should consider the population or sample being studied.  
  • Uncomplicated: A good research hypothesis is written in a way that is easy for the target audience to understand.  

By following this research hypothesis checklist , you will be able to create a research hypothesis that is strong, well-constructed, and more likely to yield meaningful results.  

Research hypothesis: What it is, how to write it, types, and examples

Types of research hypothesis  

Different types of research hypothesis are used in scientific research:  

1. Null hypothesis:

A null hypothesis states that there is no change in the dependent variable due to changes to the independent variable. This means that the results are due to chance and are not significant. A null hypothesis is denoted as H0 and is stated as the opposite of what the alternative hypothesis states.   

Example: “ The newly identified virus is not zoonotic .”  

2. Alternative hypothesis:

This states that there is a significant difference or relationship between the variables being studied. It is denoted as H1 or Ha and is usually accepted or rejected in favor of the null hypothesis.  

Example: “ The newly identified virus is zoonotic .”  

3. Directional hypothesis :

This specifies the direction of the relationship or difference between variables; therefore, it tends to use terms like increase, decrease, positive, negative, more, or less.   

Example: “ The inclusion of intervention X decreases infant mortality compared to the original treatment .”   

4. Non-directional hypothesis:

While it does not predict the exact direction or nature of the relationship between the two variables, a non-directional hypothesis states the existence of a relationship or difference between variables but not the direction, nature, or magnitude of the relationship. A non-directional hypothesis may be used when there is no underlying theory or when findings contradict previous research.  

Example, “ Cats and dogs differ in the amount of affection they express .”  

5. Simple hypothesis :

A simple hypothesis only predicts the relationship between one independent and another independent variable.  

Example: “ Applying sunscreen every day slows skin aging .”  

6 . Complex hypothesis :

A complex hypothesis states the relationship or difference between two or more independent and dependent variables.   

Example: “ Applying sunscreen every day slows skin aging, reduces sun burn, and reduces the chances of skin cancer .” (Here, the three dependent variables are slowing skin aging, reducing sun burn, and reducing the chances of skin cancer.)  

7. Associative hypothesis:  

An associative hypothesis states that a change in one variable results in the change of the other variable. The associative hypothesis defines interdependency between variables.  

Example: “ There is a positive association between physical activity levels and overall health .”  

8 . Causal hypothesis:

A causal hypothesis proposes a cause-and-effect interaction between variables.  

Example: “ Long-term alcohol use causes liver damage .”  

Note that some of the types of research hypothesis mentioned above might overlap. The types of hypothesis chosen will depend on the research question and the objective of the study.  

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

Research hypothesis examples  

Here are some good research hypothesis examples :  

“The use of a specific type of therapy will lead to a reduction in symptoms of depression in individuals with a history of major depressive disorder.”  

“Providing educational interventions on healthy eating habits will result in weight loss in overweight individuals.”  

“Plants that are exposed to certain types of music will grow taller than those that are not exposed to music.”  

“The use of the plant growth regulator X will lead to an increase in the number of flowers produced by plants.”  

Characteristics that make a research hypothesis weak are unclear variables, unoriginality, being too general or too vague, and being untestable. A weak hypothesis leads to weak research and improper methods.   

Some bad research hypothesis examples (and the reasons why they are “bad”) are as follows:  

“This study will show that treatment X is better than any other treatment . ” (This statement is not testable, too broad, and does not consider other treatments that may be effective.)  

“This study will prove that this type of therapy is effective for all mental disorders . ” (This statement is too broad and not testable as mental disorders are complex and different disorders may respond differently to different types of therapy.)  

“Plants can communicate with each other through telepathy . ” (This statement is not testable and lacks a scientific basis.)  

Importance of testable hypothesis  

If a research hypothesis is not testable, the results will not prove or disprove anything meaningful. The conclusions will be vague at best. A testable hypothesis helps a researcher focus on the study outcome and understand the implication of the question and the different variables involved. A testable hypothesis helps a researcher make precise predictions based on prior research.  

To be considered testable, there must be a way to prove that the hypothesis is true or false; further, the results of the hypothesis must be reproducible.  

Research hypothesis: What it is, how to write it, types, and examples

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs) on research hypothesis  

1. What is the difference between research question and research hypothesis ?  

A research question defines the problem and helps outline the study objective(s). It is an open-ended statement that is exploratory or probing in nature. Therefore, it does not make predictions or assumptions. It helps a researcher identify what information to collect. A research hypothesis , however, is a specific, testable prediction about the relationship between variables. Accordingly, it guides the study design and data analysis approach.

2. When to reject null hypothesis ?

A null hypothesis should be rejected when the evidence from a statistical test shows that it is unlikely to be true. This happens when the test statistic (e.g., p -value) is less than the defined significance level (e.g., 0.05). Rejecting the null hypothesis does not necessarily mean that the alternative hypothesis is true; it simply means that the evidence found is not compatible with the null hypothesis.  

3. How can I be sure my hypothesis is testable?  

A testable hypothesis should be specific and measurable, and it should state a clear relationship between variables that can be tested with data. To ensure that your hypothesis is testable, consider the following:  

  • Clearly define the key variables in your hypothesis. You should be able to measure and manipulate these variables in a way that allows you to test the hypothesis.  
  • The hypothesis should predict a specific outcome or relationship between variables that can be measured or quantified.   
  • You should be able to collect the necessary data within the constraints of your study.  
  • It should be possible for other researchers to replicate your study, using the same methods and variables.   
  • Your hypothesis should be testable by using appropriate statistical analysis techniques, so you can draw conclusions, and make inferences about the population from the sample data.  
  • The hypothesis should be able to be disproven or rejected through the collection of data.  

4. How do I revise my research hypothesis if my data does not support it?  

If your data does not support your research hypothesis , you will need to revise it or develop a new one. You should examine your data carefully and identify any patterns or anomalies, re-examine your research question, and/or revisit your theory to look for any alternative explanations for your results. Based on your review of the data, literature, and theories, modify your research hypothesis to better align it with the results you obtained. Use your revised hypothesis to guide your research design and data collection. It is important to remain objective throughout the process.  

5. I am performing exploratory research. Do I need to formulate a research hypothesis?  

As opposed to “confirmatory” research, where a researcher has some idea about the relationship between the variables under investigation, exploratory research (or hypothesis-generating research) looks into a completely new topic about which limited information is available. Therefore, the researcher will not have any prior hypotheses. In such cases, a researcher will need to develop a post-hoc hypothesis. A post-hoc research hypothesis is generated after these results are known.  

6. How is a research hypothesis different from a research question?

A research question is an inquiry about a specific topic or phenomenon, typically expressed as a question. It seeks to explore and understand a particular aspect of the research subject. In contrast, a research hypothesis is a specific statement or prediction that suggests an expected relationship between variables. It is formulated based on existing knowledge or theories and guides the research design and data analysis.

7. Can a research hypothesis change during the research process?

Yes, research hypotheses can change during the research process. As researchers collect and analyze data, new insights and information may emerge that require modification or refinement of the initial hypotheses. This can be due to unexpected findings, limitations in the original hypotheses, or the need to explore additional dimensions of the research topic. Flexibility is crucial in research, allowing for adaptation and adjustment of hypotheses to align with the evolving understanding of the subject matter.

8. How many hypotheses should be included in a research study?

The number of research hypotheses in a research study varies depending on the nature and scope of the research. It is not necessary to have multiple hypotheses in every study. Some studies may have only one primary hypothesis, while others may have several related hypotheses. The number of hypotheses should be determined based on the research objectives, research questions, and the complexity of the research topic. It is important to ensure that the hypotheses are focused, testable, and directly related to the research aims.

9. Can research hypotheses be used in qualitative research?

Yes, research hypotheses can be used in qualitative research, although they are more commonly associated with quantitative research. In qualitative research, hypotheses may be formulated as tentative or exploratory statements that guide the investigation. Instead of testing hypotheses through statistical analysis, qualitative researchers may use the hypotheses to guide data collection and analysis, seeking to uncover patterns, themes, or relationships within the qualitative data. The emphasis in qualitative research is often on generating insights and understanding rather than confirming or rejecting specific research hypotheses through statistical testing.

Editage All Access is a subscription-based platform that unifies the best AI tools and services designed to speed up, simplify, and streamline every step of a researcher’s journey. The Editage All Access Pack is a one-of-a-kind subscription that unlocks full access to an AI writing assistant, literature recommender, journal finder, scientific illustration tool, and exclusive discounts on professional publication services from Editage.  

Based on 22+ years of experience in academia, Editage All Access empowers researchers to put their best research forward and move closer to success. Explore our top AI Tools pack, AI Tools + Publication Services pack, or Build Your Own Plan. Find everything a researcher needs to succeed, all in one place –  Get All Access now starting at just $14 a month !    

Related Posts

IMRAD format

What is IMRaD Format in Research?

what is a review article

What is a Review Article? How to Write it?

If you're seeing this message, it means we're having trouble loading external resources on our website.

If you're behind a web filter, please make sure that the domains *.kastatic.org and *.kasandbox.org are unblocked.

To log in and use all the features of Khan Academy, please enable JavaScript in your browser.

Biology archive

Course: biology archive   >   unit 1.

  • The scientific method
  • Controlled experiments

The scientific method and experimental design

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

  • (Choice A)   The facts collected from an experiment are written in the form of a hypothesis. A The facts collected from an experiment are written in the form of a hypothesis.
  • (Choice B)   A hypothesis is the correct answer to a scientific question. B A hypothesis is the correct answer to a scientific question.
  • (Choice C)   A hypothesis is a possible, testable explanation for a scientific question. C A hypothesis is a possible, testable explanation for a scientific question.
  • (Choice D)   A hypothesis is the process of making careful observations. D A hypothesis is the process of making careful observations.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • J Korean Med Sci
  • v.36(50); 2021 Dec 27

Logo of jkms

Formulating Hypotheses for Different Study Designs

Durga prasanna misra.

1 Department of Clinical Immunology and Rheumatology, Sanjay Gandhi Postgraduate Institute of Medical Sciences, Lucknow, India.

Armen Yuri Gasparyan

2 Departments of Rheumatology and Research and Development, Dudley Group NHS Foundation Trust (Teaching Trust of the University of Birmingham, UK), Russells Hall Hospital, Dudley, UK.

Olena Zimba

3 Department of Internal Medicine #2, Danylo Halytsky Lviv National Medical University, Lviv, Ukraine.

Marlen Yessirkepov

4 Department of Biology and Biochemistry, South Kazakhstan Medical Academy, Shymkent, Kazakhstan.

Vikas Agarwal

George d. kitas.

5 Centre for Epidemiology versus Arthritis, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.

Generating a testable working hypothesis is the first step towards conducting original research. Such research may prove or disprove the proposed hypothesis. Case reports, case series, online surveys and other observational studies, clinical trials, and narrative reviews help to generate hypotheses. Observational and interventional studies help to test hypotheses. A good hypothesis is usually based on previous evidence-based reports. Hypotheses without evidence-based justification and a priori ideas are not received favourably by the scientific community. Original research to test a hypothesis should be carefully planned to ensure appropriate methodology and adequate statistical power. While hypotheses can challenge conventional thinking and may be controversial, they should not be destructive. A hypothesis should be tested by ethically sound experiments with meaningful ethical and clinical implications. The coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic has brought into sharp focus numerous hypotheses, some of which were proven (e.g. effectiveness of corticosteroids in those with hypoxia) while others were disproven (e.g. ineffectiveness of hydroxychloroquine and ivermectin).

Graphical Abstract

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is jkms-36-e338-abf001.jpg

DEFINING WORKING AND STANDALONE SCIENTIFIC HYPOTHESES

Science is the systematized description of natural truths and facts. Routine observations of existing life phenomena lead to the creative thinking and generation of ideas about mechanisms of such phenomena and related human interventions. Such ideas presented in a structured format can be viewed as hypotheses. After generating a hypothesis, it is necessary to test it to prove its validity. Thus, hypothesis can be defined as a proposed mechanism of a naturally occurring event or a proposed outcome of an intervention. 1 , 2

Hypothesis testing requires choosing the most appropriate methodology and adequately powering statistically the study to be able to “prove” or “disprove” it within predetermined and widely accepted levels of certainty. This entails sample size calculation that often takes into account previously published observations and pilot studies. 2 , 3 In the era of digitization, hypothesis generation and testing may benefit from the availability of numerous platforms for data dissemination, social networking, and expert validation. Related expert evaluations may reveal strengths and limitations of proposed ideas at early stages of post-publication promotion, preventing the implementation of unsupported controversial points. 4

Thus, hypothesis generation is an important initial step in the research workflow, reflecting accumulating evidence and experts' stance. In this article, we overview the genesis and importance of scientific hypotheses and their relevance in the era of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic.

DO WE NEED HYPOTHESES FOR ALL STUDY DESIGNS?

Broadly, research can be categorized as primary or secondary. In the context of medicine, primary research may include real-life observations of disease presentations and outcomes. Single case descriptions, which often lead to new ideas and hypotheses, serve as important starting points or justifications for case series and cohort studies. The importance of case descriptions is particularly evident in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic when unique, educational case reports have heralded a new era in clinical medicine. 5

Case series serve similar purpose to single case reports, but are based on a slightly larger quantum of information. Observational studies, including online surveys, describe the existing phenomena at a larger scale, often involving various control groups. Observational studies include variable-scale epidemiological investigations at different time points. Interventional studies detail the results of therapeutic interventions.

Secondary research is based on already published literature and does not directly involve human or animal subjects. Review articles are generated by secondary research. These could be systematic reviews which follow methods akin to primary research but with the unit of study being published papers rather than humans or animals. Systematic reviews have a rigid structure with a mandatory search strategy encompassing multiple databases, systematic screening of search results against pre-defined inclusion and exclusion criteria, critical appraisal of study quality and an optional component of collating results across studies quantitatively to derive summary estimates (meta-analysis). 6 Narrative reviews, on the other hand, have a more flexible structure. Systematic literature searches to minimise bias in selection of articles are highly recommended but not mandatory. 7 Narrative reviews are influenced by the authors' viewpoint who may preferentially analyse selected sets of articles. 8

In relation to primary research, case studies and case series are generally not driven by a working hypothesis. Rather, they serve as a basis to generate a hypothesis. Observational or interventional studies should have a hypothesis for choosing research design and sample size. The results of observational and interventional studies further lead to the generation of new hypotheses, testing of which forms the basis of future studies. Review articles, on the other hand, may not be hypothesis-driven, but form fertile ground to generate future hypotheses for evaluation. Fig. 1 summarizes which type of studies are hypothesis-driven and which lead on to hypothesis generation.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is jkms-36-e338-g001.jpg

STANDARDS OF WORKING AND SCIENTIFIC HYPOTHESES

A review of the published literature did not enable the identification of clearly defined standards for working and scientific hypotheses. It is essential to distinguish influential versus not influential hypotheses, evidence-based hypotheses versus a priori statements and ideas, ethical versus unethical, or potentially harmful ideas. The following points are proposed for consideration while generating working and scientific hypotheses. 1 , 2 Table 1 summarizes these points.

Points to be considered while evaluating the validity of hypotheses
Backed by evidence-based data
Testable by relevant study designs
Supported by preliminary (pilot) studies
Testable by ethical studies
Maintaining a balance between scientific temper and controversy

Evidence-based data

A scientific hypothesis should have a sound basis on previously published literature as well as the scientist's observations. Randomly generated (a priori) hypotheses are unlikely to be proven. A thorough literature search should form the basis of a hypothesis based on published evidence. 7

Unless a scientific hypothesis can be tested, it can neither be proven nor be disproven. Therefore, a scientific hypothesis should be amenable to testing with the available technologies and the present understanding of science.

Supported by pilot studies

If a hypothesis is based purely on a novel observation by the scientist in question, it should be grounded on some preliminary studies to support it. For example, if a drug that targets a specific cell population is hypothesized to be useful in a particular disease setting, then there must be some preliminary evidence that the specific cell population plays a role in driving that disease process.

Testable by ethical studies

The hypothesis should be testable by experiments that are ethically acceptable. 9 For example, a hypothesis that parachutes reduce mortality from falls from an airplane cannot be tested using a randomized controlled trial. 10 This is because it is obvious that all those jumping from a flying plane without a parachute would likely die. Similarly, the hypothesis that smoking tobacco causes lung cancer cannot be tested by a clinical trial that makes people take up smoking (since there is considerable evidence for the health hazards associated with smoking). Instead, long-term observational studies comparing outcomes in those who smoke and those who do not, as was performed in the landmark epidemiological case control study by Doll and Hill, 11 are more ethical and practical.

Balance between scientific temper and controversy

Novel findings, including novel hypotheses, particularly those that challenge established norms, are bound to face resistance for their wider acceptance. Such resistance is inevitable until the time such findings are proven with appropriate scientific rigor. However, hypotheses that generate controversy are generally unwelcome. For example, at the time the pandemic of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) and AIDS was taking foot, there were numerous deniers that refused to believe that HIV caused AIDS. 12 , 13 Similarly, at a time when climate change is causing catastrophic changes to weather patterns worldwide, denial that climate change is occurring and consequent attempts to block climate change are certainly unwelcome. 14 The denialism and misinformation during the COVID-19 pandemic, including unfortunate examples of vaccine hesitancy, are more recent examples of controversial hypotheses not backed by science. 15 , 16 An example of a controversial hypothesis that was a revolutionary scientific breakthrough was the hypothesis put forth by Warren and Marshall that Helicobacter pylori causes peptic ulcers. Initially, the hypothesis that a microorganism could cause gastritis and gastric ulcers faced immense resistance. When the scientists that proposed the hypothesis themselves ingested H. pylori to induce gastritis in themselves, only then could they convince the wider world about their hypothesis. Such was the impact of the hypothesis was that Barry Marshall and Robin Warren were awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 2005 for this discovery. 17 , 18

DISTINGUISHING THE MOST INFLUENTIAL HYPOTHESES

Influential hypotheses are those that have stood the test of time. An archetype of an influential hypothesis is that proposed by Edward Jenner in the eighteenth century that cowpox infection protects against smallpox. While this observation had been reported for nearly a century before this time, it had not been suitably tested and publicised until Jenner conducted his experiments on a young boy by demonstrating protection against smallpox after inoculation with cowpox. 19 These experiments were the basis for widespread smallpox immunization strategies worldwide in the 20th century which resulted in the elimination of smallpox as a human disease today. 20

Other influential hypotheses are those which have been read and cited widely. An example of this is the hygiene hypothesis proposing an inverse relationship between infections in early life and allergies or autoimmunity in adulthood. An analysis reported that this hypothesis had been cited more than 3,000 times on Scopus. 1

LESSONS LEARNED FROM HYPOTHESES AMIDST THE COVID-19 PANDEMIC

The COVID-19 pandemic devastated the world like no other in recent memory. During this period, various hypotheses emerged, understandably so considering the public health emergency situation with innumerable deaths and suffering for humanity. Within weeks of the first reports of COVID-19, aberrant immune system activation was identified as a key driver of organ dysfunction and mortality in this disease. 21 Consequently, numerous drugs that suppress the immune system or abrogate the activation of the immune system were hypothesized to have a role in COVID-19. 22 One of the earliest drugs hypothesized to have a benefit was hydroxychloroquine. Hydroxychloroquine was proposed to interfere with Toll-like receptor activation and consequently ameliorate the aberrant immune system activation leading to pathology in COVID-19. 22 The drug was also hypothesized to have a prophylactic role in preventing infection or disease severity in COVID-19. It was also touted as a wonder drug for the disease by many prominent international figures. However, later studies which were well-designed randomized controlled trials failed to demonstrate any benefit of hydroxychloroquine in COVID-19. 23 , 24 , 25 , 26 Subsequently, azithromycin 27 , 28 and ivermectin 29 were hypothesized as potential therapies for COVID-19, but were not supported by evidence from randomized controlled trials. The role of vitamin D in preventing disease severity was also proposed, but has not been proven definitively until now. 30 , 31 On the other hand, randomized controlled trials identified the evidence supporting dexamethasone 32 and interleukin-6 pathway blockade with tocilizumab as effective therapies for COVID-19 in specific situations such as at the onset of hypoxia. 33 , 34 Clues towards the apparent effectiveness of various drugs against severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 in vitro but their ineffectiveness in vivo have recently been identified. Many of these drugs are weak, lipophilic bases and some others induce phospholipidosis which results in apparent in vitro effectiveness due to non-specific off-target effects that are not replicated inside living systems. 35 , 36

Another hypothesis proposed was the association of the routine policy of vaccination with Bacillus Calmette-Guerin (BCG) with lower deaths due to COVID-19. This hypothesis emerged in the middle of 2020 when COVID-19 was still taking foot in many parts of the world. 37 , 38 Subsequently, many countries which had lower deaths at that time point went on to have higher numbers of mortality, comparable to other areas of the world. Furthermore, the hypothesis that BCG vaccination reduced COVID-19 mortality was a classic example of ecological fallacy. Associations between population level events (ecological studies; in this case, BCG vaccination and COVID-19 mortality) cannot be directly extrapolated to the individual level. Furthermore, such associations cannot per se be attributed as causal in nature, and can only serve to generate hypotheses that need to be tested at the individual level. 39

IS TRADITIONAL PEER REVIEW EFFICIENT FOR EVALUATION OF WORKING AND SCIENTIFIC HYPOTHESES?

Traditionally, publication after peer review has been considered the gold standard before any new idea finds acceptability amongst the scientific community. Getting a work (including a working or scientific hypothesis) reviewed by experts in the field before experiments are conducted to prove or disprove it helps to refine the idea further as well as improve the experiments planned to test the hypothesis. 40 A route towards this has been the emergence of journals dedicated to publishing hypotheses such as the Central Asian Journal of Medical Hypotheses and Ethics. 41 Another means of publishing hypotheses is through registered research protocols detailing the background, hypothesis, and methodology of a particular study. If such protocols are published after peer review, then the journal commits to publishing the completed study irrespective of whether the study hypothesis is proven or disproven. 42 In the post-pandemic world, online research methods such as online surveys powered via social media channels such as Twitter and Instagram might serve as critical tools to generate as well as to preliminarily test the appropriateness of hypotheses for further evaluation. 43 , 44

Some radical hypotheses might be difficult to publish after traditional peer review. These hypotheses might only be acceptable by the scientific community after they are tested in research studies. Preprints might be a way to disseminate such controversial and ground-breaking hypotheses. 45 However, scientists might prefer to keep their hypotheses confidential for the fear of plagiarism of ideas, avoiding online posting and publishing until they have tested the hypotheses.

SUGGESTIONS ON GENERATING AND PUBLISHING HYPOTHESES

Publication of hypotheses is important, however, a balance is required between scientific temper and controversy. Journal editors and reviewers might keep in mind these specific points, summarized in Table 2 and detailed hereafter, while judging the merit of hypotheses for publication. Keeping in mind the ethical principle of primum non nocere, a hypothesis should be published only if it is testable in a manner that is ethically appropriate. 46 Such hypotheses should be grounded in reality and lend themselves to further testing to either prove or disprove them. It must be considered that subsequent experiments to prove or disprove a hypothesis have an equal chance of failing or succeeding, akin to tossing a coin. A pre-conceived belief that a hypothesis is unlikely to be proven correct should not form the basis of rejection of such a hypothesis for publication. In this context, hypotheses generated after a thorough literature search to identify knowledge gaps or based on concrete clinical observations on a considerable number of patients (as opposed to random observations on a few patients) are more likely to be acceptable for publication by peer-reviewed journals. Also, hypotheses should be considered for publication or rejection based on their implications for science at large rather than whether the subsequent experiments to test them end up with results in favour of or against the original hypothesis.

Points to be considered before a hypothesis is acceptable for publication
Experiments required to test hypotheses should be ethically acceptable as per the World Medical Association declaration on ethics and related statements
Pilot studies support hypotheses
Single clinical observations and expert opinion surveys may support hypotheses
Testing hypotheses requires robust methodology and statistical power
Hypotheses that challenge established views and concepts require proper evidence-based justification

Hypotheses form an important part of the scientific literature. The COVID-19 pandemic has reiterated the importance and relevance of hypotheses for dealing with public health emergencies and highlighted the need for evidence-based and ethical hypotheses. A good hypothesis is testable in a relevant study design, backed by preliminary evidence, and has positive ethical and clinical implications. General medical journals might consider publishing hypotheses as a specific article type to enable more rapid advancement of science.

Disclosure: The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions:

  • Data curation: Gasparyan AY, Misra DP, Zimba O, Yessirkepov M, Agarwal V, Kitas GD.

Module 1: Introduction to Biology

Experiments and hypotheses, learning outcomes.

  • Form a hypothesis and use it to design a scientific experiment

Now we’ll focus on the methods of scientific inquiry. Science often involves making observations and developing hypotheses. Experiments and further observations are often used to test the hypotheses.

A scientific experiment is a carefully organized procedure in which the scientist intervenes in a system to change something, then observes the result of the change. Scientific inquiry often involves doing experiments, though not always. For example, a scientist studying the mating behaviors of ladybugs might begin with detailed observations of ladybugs mating in their natural habitats. While this research may not be experimental, it is scientific: it involves careful and verifiable observation of the natural world. The same scientist might then treat some of the ladybugs with a hormone hypothesized to trigger mating and observe whether these ladybugs mated sooner or more often than untreated ones. This would qualify as an experiment because the scientist is now making a change in the system and observing the effects.

Forming a Hypothesis

When conducting scientific experiments, researchers develop hypotheses to guide experimental design. A hypothesis is a suggested explanation that is both testable and falsifiable. You must be able to test your hypothesis, and it must be possible to prove your hypothesis true or false.

For example, Michael observes that maple trees lose their leaves in the fall. He might then propose a possible explanation for this observation: “cold weather causes maple trees to lose their leaves in the fall.” This statement is testable. He could grow maple trees in a warm enclosed environment such as a greenhouse and see if their leaves still dropped in the fall. The hypothesis is also falsifiable. If the leaves still dropped in the warm environment, then clearly temperature was not the main factor in causing maple leaves to drop in autumn.

In the Try It below, you can practice recognizing scientific hypotheses. As you consider each statement, try to think as a scientist would: can I test this hypothesis with observations or experiments? Is the statement falsifiable? If the answer to either of these questions is “no,” the statement is not a valid scientific hypothesis.

Practice Questions

Determine whether each following statement is a scientific hypothesis.

Air pollution from automobile exhaust can trigger symptoms in people with asthma.

  • No. This statement is not testable or falsifiable.
  • No. This statement is not testable.
  • No. This statement is not falsifiable.
  • Yes. This statement is testable and falsifiable.

Natural disasters, such as tornadoes, are punishments for bad thoughts and behaviors.

a: No. This statement is not testable or falsifiable. “Bad thoughts and behaviors” are excessively vague and subjective variables that would be impossible to measure or agree upon in a reliable way. The statement might be “falsifiable” if you came up with a counterexample: a “wicked” place that was not punished by a natural disaster. But some would question whether the people in that place were really wicked, and others would continue to predict that a natural disaster was bound to strike that place at some point. There is no reason to suspect that people’s immoral behavior affects the weather unless you bring up the intervention of a supernatural being, making this idea even harder to test.

Testing a Vaccine

Let’s examine the scientific process by discussing an actual scientific experiment conducted by researchers at the University of Washington. These researchers investigated whether a vaccine may reduce the incidence of the human papillomavirus (HPV). The experimental process and results were published in an article titled, “ A controlled trial of a human papillomavirus type 16 vaccine .”

Preliminary observations made by the researchers who conducted the HPV experiment are listed below:

  • Human papillomavirus (HPV) is the most common sexually transmitted virus in the United States.
  • There are about 40 different types of HPV. A significant number of people that have HPV are unaware of it because many of these viruses cause no symptoms.
  • Some types of HPV can cause cervical cancer.
  • About 4,000 women a year die of cervical cancer in the United States.

Practice Question

Researchers have developed a potential vaccine against HPV and want to test it. What is the first testable hypothesis that the researchers should study?

  • HPV causes cervical cancer.
  • People should not have unprotected sex with many partners.
  • People who get the vaccine will not get HPV.
  • The HPV vaccine will protect people against cancer.

Experimental Design

You’ve successfully identified a hypothesis for the University of Washington’s study on HPV: People who get the HPV vaccine will not get HPV.

The next step is to design an experiment that will test this hypothesis. There are several important factors to consider when designing a scientific experiment. First, scientific experiments must have an experimental group. This is the group that receives the experimental treatment necessary to address the hypothesis.

The experimental group receives the vaccine, but how can we know if the vaccine made a difference? Many things may change HPV infection rates in a group of people over time. To clearly show that the vaccine was effective in helping the experimental group, we need to include in our study an otherwise similar control group that does not get the treatment. We can then compare the two groups and determine if the vaccine made a difference. The control group shows us what happens in the absence of the factor under study.

However, the control group cannot get “nothing.” Instead, the control group often receives a placebo. A placebo is a procedure that has no expected therapeutic effect—such as giving a person a sugar pill or a shot containing only plain saline solution with no drug. Scientific studies have shown that the “placebo effect” can alter experimental results because when individuals are told that they are or are not being treated, this knowledge can alter their actions or their emotions, which can then alter the results of the experiment.

Moreover, if the doctor knows which group a patient is in, this can also influence the results of the experiment. Without saying so directly, the doctor may show—through body language or other subtle cues—their views about whether the patient is likely to get well. These errors can then alter the patient’s experience and change the results of the experiment. Therefore, many clinical studies are “double blind.” In these studies, neither the doctor nor the patient knows which group the patient is in until all experimental results have been collected.

Both placebo treatments and double-blind procedures are designed to prevent bias. Bias is any systematic error that makes a particular experimental outcome more or less likely. Errors can happen in any experiment: people make mistakes in measurement, instruments fail, computer glitches can alter data. But most such errors are random and don’t favor one outcome over another. Patients’ belief in a treatment can make it more likely to appear to “work.” Placebos and double-blind procedures are used to level the playing field so that both groups of study subjects are treated equally and share similar beliefs about their treatment.

The scientists who are researching the effectiveness of the HPV vaccine will test their hypothesis by separating 2,392 young women into two groups: the control group and the experimental group. Answer the following questions about these two groups.

  • This group is given a placebo.
  • This group is deliberately infected with HPV.
  • This group is given nothing.
  • This group is given the HPV vaccine.
  • a: This group is given a placebo. A placebo will be a shot, just like the HPV vaccine, but it will have no active ingredient. It may change peoples’ thinking or behavior to have such a shot given to them, but it will not stimulate the immune systems of the subjects in the same way as predicted for the vaccine itself.
  • d: This group is given the HPV vaccine. The experimental group will receive the HPV vaccine and researchers will then be able to see if it works, when compared to the control group.

Experimental Variables

A variable is a characteristic of a subject (in this case, of a person in the study) that can vary over time or among individuals. Sometimes a variable takes the form of a category, such as male or female; often a variable can be measured precisely, such as body height. Ideally, only one variable is different between the control group and the experimental group in a scientific experiment. Otherwise, the researchers will not be able to determine which variable caused any differences seen in the results. For example, imagine that the people in the control group were, on average, much more sexually active than the people in the experimental group. If, at the end of the experiment, the control group had a higher rate of HPV infection, could you confidently determine why? Maybe the experimental subjects were protected by the vaccine, but maybe they were protected by their low level of sexual contact.

To avoid this situation, experimenters make sure that their subject groups are as similar as possible in all variables except for the variable that is being tested in the experiment. This variable, or factor, will be deliberately changed in the experimental group. The one variable that is different between the two groups is called the independent variable. An independent variable is known or hypothesized to cause some outcome. Imagine an educational researcher investigating the effectiveness of a new teaching strategy in a classroom. The experimental group receives the new teaching strategy, while the control group receives the traditional strategy. It is the teaching strategy that is the independent variable in this scenario. In an experiment, the independent variable is the variable that the scientist deliberately changes or imposes on the subjects.

Dependent variables are known or hypothesized consequences; they are the effects that result from changes or differences in an independent variable. In an experiment, the dependent variables are those that the scientist measures before, during, and particularly at the end of the experiment to see if they have changed as expected. The dependent variable must be stated so that it is clear how it will be observed or measured. Rather than comparing “learning” among students (which is a vague and difficult to measure concept), an educational researcher might choose to compare test scores, which are very specific and easy to measure.

In any real-world example, many, many variables MIGHT affect the outcome of an experiment, yet only one or a few independent variables can be tested. Other variables must be kept as similar as possible between the study groups and are called control variables . For our educational research example, if the control group consisted only of people between the ages of 18 and 20 and the experimental group contained people between the ages of 30 and 35, we would not know if it was the teaching strategy or the students’ ages that played a larger role in the results. To avoid this problem, a good study will be set up so that each group contains students with a similar age profile. In a well-designed educational research study, student age will be a controlled variable, along with other possibly important factors like gender, past educational achievement, and pre-existing knowledge of the subject area.

What is the independent variable in this experiment?

  • Sex (all of the subjects will be female)
  • Presence or absence of the HPV vaccine
  • Presence or absence of HPV (the virus)

List three control variables other than age.

What is the dependent variable in this experiment?

  • Sex (male or female)
  • Rates of HPV infection
  • Age (years)
  • Revision and adaptation. Authored by : Shelli Carter and Lumen Learning. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY-NC-SA: Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
  • Scientific Inquiry. Provided by : Open Learning Initiative. Located at : https://oli.cmu.edu/jcourse/workbook/activity/page?context=434a5c2680020ca6017c03488572e0f8 . Project : Introduction to Biology (Open + Free). License : CC BY-NC-SA: Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike

Footer Logo Lumen Waymaker

Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base

Hypothesis Testing | A Step-by-Step Guide with Easy Examples

Published on November 8, 2019 by Rebecca Bevans . Revised on June 22, 2023.

Hypothesis testing is a formal procedure for investigating our ideas about the world using statistics . It is most often used by scientists to test specific predictions, called hypotheses, that arise from theories.

There are 5 main steps in hypothesis testing:

  • State your research hypothesis as a null hypothesis and alternate hypothesis (H o ) and (H a  or H 1 ).
  • Collect data in a way designed to test the hypothesis.
  • Perform an appropriate statistical test .
  • Decide whether to reject or fail to reject your null hypothesis.
  • Present the findings in your results and discussion section.

Though the specific details might vary, the procedure you will use when testing a hypothesis will always follow some version of these steps.

Table of contents

Step 1: state your null and alternate hypothesis, step 2: collect data, step 3: perform a statistical test, step 4: decide whether to reject or fail to reject your null hypothesis, step 5: present your findings, other interesting articles, frequently asked questions about hypothesis testing.

After developing your initial research hypothesis (the prediction that you want to investigate), it is important to restate it as a null (H o ) and alternate (H a ) hypothesis so that you can test it mathematically.

The alternate hypothesis is usually your initial hypothesis that predicts a relationship between variables. The null hypothesis is a prediction of no relationship between the variables you are interested in.

  • H 0 : Men are, on average, not taller than women. H a : Men are, on average, taller than women.

Here's why students love Scribbr's proofreading services

Discover proofreading & editing

For a statistical test to be valid , it is important to perform sampling and collect data in a way that is designed to test your hypothesis. If your data are not representative, then you cannot make statistical inferences about the population you are interested in.

There are a variety of statistical tests available, but they are all based on the comparison of within-group variance (how spread out the data is within a category) versus between-group variance (how different the categories are from one another).

If the between-group variance is large enough that there is little or no overlap between groups, then your statistical test will reflect that by showing a low p -value . This means it is unlikely that the differences between these groups came about by chance.

Alternatively, if there is high within-group variance and low between-group variance, then your statistical test will reflect that with a high p -value. This means it is likely that any difference you measure between groups is due to chance.

Your choice of statistical test will be based on the type of variables and the level of measurement of your collected data .

  • an estimate of the difference in average height between the two groups.
  • a p -value showing how likely you are to see this difference if the null hypothesis of no difference is true.

Based on the outcome of your statistical test, you will have to decide whether to reject or fail to reject your null hypothesis.

In most cases you will use the p -value generated by your statistical test to guide your decision. And in most cases, your predetermined level of significance for rejecting the null hypothesis will be 0.05 – that is, when there is a less than 5% chance that you would see these results if the null hypothesis were true.

In some cases, researchers choose a more conservative level of significance, such as 0.01 (1%). This minimizes the risk of incorrectly rejecting the null hypothesis ( Type I error ).

Receive feedback on language, structure, and formatting

Professional editors proofread and edit your paper by focusing on:

  • Academic style
  • Vague sentences
  • Style consistency

See an example

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

The results of hypothesis testing will be presented in the results and discussion sections of your research paper , dissertation or thesis .

In the results section you should give a brief summary of the data and a summary of the results of your statistical test (for example, the estimated difference between group means and associated p -value). In the discussion , you can discuss whether your initial hypothesis was supported by your results or not.

In the formal language of hypothesis testing, we talk about rejecting or failing to reject the null hypothesis. You will probably be asked to do this in your statistics assignments.

However, when presenting research results in academic papers we rarely talk this way. Instead, we go back to our alternate hypothesis (in this case, the hypothesis that men are on average taller than women) and state whether the result of our test did or did not support the alternate hypothesis.

If your null hypothesis was rejected, this result is interpreted as “supported the alternate hypothesis.”

These are superficial differences; you can see that they mean the same thing.

You might notice that we don’t say that we reject or fail to reject the alternate hypothesis . This is because hypothesis testing is not designed to prove or disprove anything. It is only designed to test whether a pattern we measure could have arisen spuriously, or by chance.

If we reject the null hypothesis based on our research (i.e., we find that it is unlikely that the pattern arose by chance), then we can say our test lends support to our hypothesis . But if the pattern does not pass our decision rule, meaning that it could have arisen by chance, then we say the test is inconsistent with our hypothesis .

If you want to know more about statistics , methodology , or research bias , make sure to check out some of our other articles with explanations and examples.

  • Normal distribution
  • Descriptive statistics
  • Measures of central tendency
  • Correlation coefficient

Methodology

  • Cluster sampling
  • Stratified sampling
  • Types of interviews
  • Cohort study
  • Thematic analysis

Research bias

  • Implicit bias
  • Cognitive bias
  • Survivorship bias
  • Availability heuristic
  • Nonresponse bias
  • Regression to the mean

Hypothesis testing is a formal procedure for investigating our ideas about the world using statistics. It is used by scientists to test specific predictions, called hypotheses , by calculating how likely it is that a pattern or relationship between variables could have arisen by chance.

A hypothesis states your predictions about what your research will find. It is a tentative answer to your research question that has not yet been tested. For some research projects, you might have to write several hypotheses that address different aspects of your research question.

A hypothesis is not just a guess — it should be based on existing theories and knowledge. It also has to be testable, which means you can support or refute it through scientific research methods (such as experiments, observations and statistical analysis of data).

Null and alternative hypotheses are used in statistical hypothesis testing . The null hypothesis of a test always predicts no effect or no relationship between variables, while the alternative hypothesis states your research prediction of an effect or relationship.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.

Bevans, R. (2023, June 22). Hypothesis Testing | A Step-by-Step Guide with Easy Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved August 10, 2024, from https://www.scribbr.com/statistics/hypothesis-testing/

Is this article helpful?

Rebecca Bevans

Rebecca Bevans

Other students also liked, choosing the right statistical test | types & examples, understanding p values | definition and examples, what is your plagiarism score.

  • Resources Home 🏠
  • Try SciSpace Copilot
  • Search research papers
  • Add Copilot Extension
  • Try AI Detector
  • Try Paraphraser
  • Try Citation Generator
  • April Papers
  • June Papers
  • July Papers

SciSpace Resources

The Craft of Writing a Strong Hypothesis

Deeptanshu D

Table of Contents

Writing a hypothesis is one of the essential elements of a scientific research paper. It needs to be to the point, clearly communicating what your research is trying to accomplish. A blurry, drawn-out, or complexly-structured hypothesis can confuse your readers. Or worse, the editor and peer reviewers.

A captivating hypothesis is not too intricate. This blog will take you through the process so that, by the end of it, you have a better idea of how to convey your research paper's intent in just one sentence.

What is a Hypothesis?

The first step in your scientific endeavor, a hypothesis, is a strong, concise statement that forms the basis of your research. It is not the same as a thesis statement , which is a brief summary of your research paper .

The sole purpose of a hypothesis is to predict your paper's findings, data, and conclusion. It comes from a place of curiosity and intuition . When you write a hypothesis, you're essentially making an educated guess based on scientific prejudices and evidence, which is further proven or disproven through the scientific method.

The reason for undertaking research is to observe a specific phenomenon. A hypothesis, therefore, lays out what the said phenomenon is. And it does so through two variables, an independent and dependent variable.

The independent variable is the cause behind the observation, while the dependent variable is the effect of the cause. A good example of this is “mixing red and blue forms purple.” In this hypothesis, mixing red and blue is the independent variable as you're combining the two colors at your own will. The formation of purple is the dependent variable as, in this case, it is conditional to the independent variable.

Different Types of Hypotheses‌

Types-of-hypotheses

Types of hypotheses

Some would stand by the notion that there are only two types of hypotheses: a Null hypothesis and an Alternative hypothesis. While that may have some truth to it, it would be better to fully distinguish the most common forms as these terms come up so often, which might leave you out of context.

Apart from Null and Alternative, there are Complex, Simple, Directional, Non-Directional, Statistical, and Associative and casual hypotheses. They don't necessarily have to be exclusive, as one hypothesis can tick many boxes, but knowing the distinctions between them will make it easier for you to construct your own.

1. Null hypothesis

A null hypothesis proposes no relationship between two variables. Denoted by H 0 , it is a negative statement like “Attending physiotherapy sessions does not affect athletes' on-field performance.” Here, the author claims physiotherapy sessions have no effect on on-field performances. Even if there is, it's only a coincidence.

2. Alternative hypothesis

Considered to be the opposite of a null hypothesis, an alternative hypothesis is donated as H1 or Ha. It explicitly states that the dependent variable affects the independent variable. A good  alternative hypothesis example is “Attending physiotherapy sessions improves athletes' on-field performance.” or “Water evaporates at 100 °C. ” The alternative hypothesis further branches into directional and non-directional.

  • Directional hypothesis: A hypothesis that states the result would be either positive or negative is called directional hypothesis. It accompanies H1 with either the ‘<' or ‘>' sign.
  • Non-directional hypothesis: A non-directional hypothesis only claims an effect on the dependent variable. It does not clarify whether the result would be positive or negative. The sign for a non-directional hypothesis is ‘≠.'

3. Simple hypothesis

A simple hypothesis is a statement made to reflect the relation between exactly two variables. One independent and one dependent. Consider the example, “Smoking is a prominent cause of lung cancer." The dependent variable, lung cancer, is dependent on the independent variable, smoking.

4. Complex hypothesis

In contrast to a simple hypothesis, a complex hypothesis implies the relationship between multiple independent and dependent variables. For instance, “Individuals who eat more fruits tend to have higher immunity, lesser cholesterol, and high metabolism.” The independent variable is eating more fruits, while the dependent variables are higher immunity, lesser cholesterol, and high metabolism.

5. Associative and casual hypothesis

Associative and casual hypotheses don't exhibit how many variables there will be. They define the relationship between the variables. In an associative hypothesis, changing any one variable, dependent or independent, affects others. In a casual hypothesis, the independent variable directly affects the dependent.

6. Empirical hypothesis

Also referred to as the working hypothesis, an empirical hypothesis claims a theory's validation via experiments and observation. This way, the statement appears justifiable and different from a wild guess.

Say, the hypothesis is “Women who take iron tablets face a lesser risk of anemia than those who take vitamin B12.” This is an example of an empirical hypothesis where the researcher  the statement after assessing a group of women who take iron tablets and charting the findings.

7. Statistical hypothesis

The point of a statistical hypothesis is to test an already existing hypothesis by studying a population sample. Hypothesis like “44% of the Indian population belong in the age group of 22-27.” leverage evidence to prove or disprove a particular statement.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

Writing a hypothesis is essential as it can make or break your research for you. That includes your chances of getting published in a journal. So when you're designing one, keep an eye out for these pointers:

  • A research hypothesis has to be simple yet clear to look justifiable enough.
  • It has to be testable — your research would be rendered pointless if too far-fetched into reality or limited by technology.
  • It has to be precise about the results —what you are trying to do and achieve through it should come out in your hypothesis.
  • A research hypothesis should be self-explanatory, leaving no doubt in the reader's mind.
  • If you are developing a relational hypothesis, you need to include the variables and establish an appropriate relationship among them.
  • A hypothesis must keep and reflect the scope for further investigations and experiments.

Separating a Hypothesis from a Prediction

Outside of academia, hypothesis and prediction are often used interchangeably. In research writing, this is not only confusing but also incorrect. And although a hypothesis and prediction are guesses at their core, there are many differences between them.

A hypothesis is an educated guess or even a testable prediction validated through research. It aims to analyze the gathered evidence and facts to define a relationship between variables and put forth a logical explanation behind the nature of events.

Predictions are assumptions or expected outcomes made without any backing evidence. They are more fictionally inclined regardless of where they originate from.

For this reason, a hypothesis holds much more weight than a prediction. It sticks to the scientific method rather than pure guesswork. "Planets revolve around the Sun." is an example of a hypothesis as it is previous knowledge and observed trends. Additionally, we can test it through the scientific method.

Whereas "COVID-19 will be eradicated by 2030." is a prediction. Even though it results from past trends, we can't prove or disprove it. So, the only way this gets validated is to wait and watch if COVID-19 cases end by 2030.

Finally, How to Write a Hypothesis

Quick-tips-on-how-to-write-a-hypothesis

Quick tips on writing a hypothesis

1.  Be clear about your research question

A hypothesis should instantly address the research question or the problem statement. To do so, you need to ask a question. Understand the constraints of your undertaken research topic and then formulate a simple and topic-centric problem. Only after that can you develop a hypothesis and further test for evidence.

2. Carry out a recce

Once you have your research's foundation laid out, it would be best to conduct preliminary research. Go through previous theories, academic papers, data, and experiments before you start curating your research hypothesis. It will give you an idea of your hypothesis's viability or originality.

Making use of references from relevant research papers helps draft a good research hypothesis. SciSpace Discover offers a repository of over 270 million research papers to browse through and gain a deeper understanding of related studies on a particular topic. Additionally, you can use SciSpace Copilot , your AI research assistant, for reading any lengthy research paper and getting a more summarized context of it. A hypothesis can be formed after evaluating many such summarized research papers. Copilot also offers explanations for theories and equations, explains paper in simplified version, allows you to highlight any text in the paper or clip math equations and tables and provides a deeper, clear understanding of what is being said. This can improve the hypothesis by helping you identify potential research gaps.

3. Create a 3-dimensional hypothesis

Variables are an essential part of any reasonable hypothesis. So, identify your independent and dependent variable(s) and form a correlation between them. The ideal way to do this is to write the hypothetical assumption in the ‘if-then' form. If you use this form, make sure that you state the predefined relationship between the variables.

In another way, you can choose to present your hypothesis as a comparison between two variables. Here, you must specify the difference you expect to observe in the results.

4. Write the first draft

Now that everything is in place, it's time to write your hypothesis. For starters, create the first draft. In this version, write what you expect to find from your research.

Clearly separate your independent and dependent variables and the link between them. Don't fixate on syntax at this stage. The goal is to ensure your hypothesis addresses the issue.

5. Proof your hypothesis

After preparing the first draft of your hypothesis, you need to inspect it thoroughly. It should tick all the boxes, like being concise, straightforward, relevant, and accurate. Your final hypothesis has to be well-structured as well.

Research projects are an exciting and crucial part of being a scholar. And once you have your research question, you need a great hypothesis to begin conducting research. Thus, knowing how to write a hypothesis is very important.

Now that you have a firmer grasp on what a good hypothesis constitutes, the different kinds there are, and what process to follow, you will find it much easier to write your hypothesis, which ultimately helps your research.

Now it's easier than ever to streamline your research workflow with SciSpace Discover . Its integrated, comprehensive end-to-end platform for research allows scholars to easily discover, write and publish their research and fosters collaboration.

It includes everything you need, including a repository of over 270 million research papers across disciplines, SEO-optimized summaries and public profiles to show your expertise and experience.

If you found these tips on writing a research hypothesis useful, head over to our blog on Statistical Hypothesis Testing to learn about the top researchers, papers, and institutions in this domain.

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs)

1. what is the definition of hypothesis.

According to the Oxford dictionary, a hypothesis is defined as “An idea or explanation of something that is based on a few known facts, but that has not yet been proved to be true or correct”.

2. What is an example of hypothesis?

The hypothesis is a statement that proposes a relationship between two or more variables. An example: "If we increase the number of new users who join our platform by 25%, then we will see an increase in revenue."

3. What is an example of null hypothesis?

A null hypothesis is a statement that there is no relationship between two variables. The null hypothesis is written as H0. The null hypothesis states that there is no effect. For example, if you're studying whether or not a particular type of exercise increases strength, your null hypothesis will be "there is no difference in strength between people who exercise and people who don't."

4. What are the types of research?

• Fundamental research

• Applied research

• Qualitative research

• Quantitative research

• Mixed research

• Exploratory research

• Longitudinal research

• Cross-sectional research

• Field research

• Laboratory research

• Fixed research

• Flexible research

• Action research

• Policy research

• Classification research

• Comparative research

• Causal research

• Inductive research

• Deductive research

5. How to write a hypothesis?

• Your hypothesis should be able to predict the relationship and outcome.

• Avoid wordiness by keeping it simple and brief.

• Your hypothesis should contain observable and testable outcomes.

• Your hypothesis should be relevant to the research question.

6. What are the 2 types of hypothesis?

• Null hypotheses are used to test the claim that "there is no difference between two groups of data".

• Alternative hypotheses test the claim that "there is a difference between two data groups".

7. Difference between research question and research hypothesis?

A research question is a broad, open-ended question you will try to answer through your research. A hypothesis is a statement based on prior research or theory that you expect to be true due to your study. Example - Research question: What are the factors that influence the adoption of the new technology? Research hypothesis: There is a positive relationship between age, education and income level with the adoption of the new technology.

8. What is plural for hypothesis?

The plural of hypothesis is hypotheses. Here's an example of how it would be used in a statement, "Numerous well-considered hypotheses are presented in this part, and they are supported by tables and figures that are well-illustrated."

9. What is the red queen hypothesis?

The red queen hypothesis in evolutionary biology states that species must constantly evolve to avoid extinction because if they don't, they will be outcompeted by other species that are evolving. Leigh Van Valen first proposed it in 1973; since then, it has been tested and substantiated many times.

10. Who is known as the father of null hypothesis?

The father of the null hypothesis is Sir Ronald Fisher. He published a paper in 1925 that introduced the concept of null hypothesis testing, and he was also the first to use the term itself.

11. When to reject null hypothesis?

You need to find a significant difference between your two populations to reject the null hypothesis. You can determine that by running statistical tests such as an independent sample t-test or a dependent sample t-test. You should reject the null hypothesis if the p-value is less than 0.05.

an experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement

You might also like

Consensus GPT vs. SciSpace GPT: Choose the Best GPT for Research

Consensus GPT vs. SciSpace GPT: Choose the Best GPT for Research

Sumalatha G

Literature Review and Theoretical Framework: Understanding the Differences

Nikhil Seethi

Types of Essays in Academic Writing - Quick Guide (2024)

What Is a Testable Hypothesis?

  • Scientific Method
  • Chemical Laws
  • Periodic Table
  • Projects & Experiments
  • Biochemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Medical Chemistry
  • Chemistry In Everyday Life
  • Famous Chemists
  • Activities for Kids
  • Abbreviations & Acronyms
  • Weather & Climate
  • Ph.D., Biomedical Sciences, University of Tennessee at Knoxville
  • B.A., Physics and Mathematics, Hastings College

A hypothesis is a tentative answer to a scientific question. A testable hypothesis is a  hypothesis that can be proved or disproved as a result of testing, data collection, or experience. Only testable hypotheses can be used to conceive and perform an experiment using the scientific method .

Requirements for a Testable Hypothesis

In order to be considered testable, two criteria must be met:

  • It must be possible to prove that the hypothesis is true.
  • It must be possible to prove that the hypothesis is false.
  • It must be possible to reproduce the results of the hypothesis.

Examples of a Testable Hypothesis

All the following hypotheses are testable. It's important, however, to note that while it's possible to say that the hypothesis is correct, much more research would be required to answer the question " why is this hypothesis correct?" 

  • Students who attend class have higher grades than students who skip class.  This is testable because it is possible to compare the grades of students who do and do not skip class and then analyze the resulting data. Another person could conduct the same research and come up with the same results.
  • People exposed to high levels of ultraviolet light have a higher incidence of cancer than the norm.  This is testable because it is possible to find a group of people who have been exposed to high levels of ultraviolet light and compare their cancer rates to the average.
  • If you put people in a dark room, then they will be unable to tell when an infrared light turns on.  This hypothesis is testable because it is possible to put a group of people into a dark room, turn on an infrared light, and ask the people in the room whether or not an infrared light has been turned on.

Examples of a Hypothesis Not Written in a Testable Form

  • It doesn't matter whether or not you skip class.  This hypothesis can't be tested because it doesn't make any actual claim regarding the outcome of skipping class. "It doesn't matter" doesn't have any specific meaning, so it can't be tested.
  • Ultraviolet light could cause cancer.  The word "could" makes a hypothesis extremely difficult to test because it is very vague. There "could," for example, be UFOs watching us at every moment, even though it's impossible to prove that they are there!
  • Goldfish make better pets than guinea pigs.  This is not a hypothesis; it's a matter of opinion. There is no agreed-upon definition of what a "better" pet is, so while it is possible to argue the point, there is no way to prove it.

How to Propose a Testable Hypothesis

Now that you know what a testable hypothesis is, here are tips for proposing one.

  • Try to write the hypothesis as an if-then statement. If you take an action, then a certain outcome is expected.
  • Identify the independent and dependent variable in the hypothesis. The independent variable is what you are controlling or changing. You measure the effect this has on the dependent variable.
  • Write the hypothesis in such a way that you can prove or disprove it. For example, a person has skin cancer, you can't prove they got it from being out in the sun. However, you can demonstrate a relationship between exposure to ultraviolet light and increased risk of skin cancer.
  • Make sure you are proposing a hypothesis you can test with reproducible results. If your face breaks out, you can't prove the breakout was caused by the french fries you had for dinner last night. However, you can measure whether or not eating french fries is associated with breaking out. It's a matter of gathering enough data to be able to reproduce results and draw a conclusion.
  • What Are Examples of a Hypothesis?
  • What Is a Hypothesis? (Science)
  • What Are the Elements of a Good Hypothesis?
  • Scientific Method Flow Chart
  • Null Hypothesis Examples
  • Scientific Hypothesis Examples
  • Understanding Simple vs Controlled Experiments
  • Six Steps of the Scientific Method
  • Scientific Method Vocabulary Terms
  • What Is a Controlled Experiment?
  • What Is an Experimental Constant?
  • Scientific Variable
  • What Is the Difference Between a Control Variable and Control Group?
  • DRY MIX Experiment Variables Acronym
  • Random Error vs. Systematic Error
  • The Role of a Controlled Variable in an Experiment

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here .

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

#ForYou? the impact of pro-ana TikTok content on body image dissatisfaction and internalisation of societal beauty standards

Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliation Faculty of Business, School of Psychology, Justice and Behavioural Science, Charles Sturt University, Wagga Wagga, New South Wales, Australia

Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

* E-mail: [email protected]

ORCID logo

  • Madison R. Blackburn, 
  • Rachel C. Hogg

PLOS

  • Published: August 7, 2024
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597
  • Peer Review
  • Reader Comments

Table 1

Videos glamourising disordered eating practices and body image concerns readily circulate on TikTok. Minimal empirical research has investigated the impact of TikTok content on body image and eating behaviour. The present study aimed to fill this gap in current research by examining the influence of pro-anorexia TikTok content on young women’s body image and degree of internalisation of beauty standards, whilst also exploring the impact of daily time spent on TikTok and the development of disordered eating behaviours. An experimental and cross-sectional design was used to explore body image and internalisation of beauty standards in relation to pro-anorexia TikTok content. Time spent on TikTok was examined in relation to the risk of developing orthorexia nervosa. A sample of 273 female-identifying persons aged 18–28 years were exposed to either pro-anorexia or neutral TikTok content. Pre- and post-test measures of body image and internalisation of beauty standards were obtained. Participants were divided into four groups based on average daily time spent on TikTok. Women exposed to pro-anorexia content displayed the greatest decrease in body image satisfaction and an increase in internalisation of societal beauty standards. Women exposed to neutral content also reported a decrease in body image satisfaction. Participants categorised as high and extreme daily TikTok users reported greater average disordered eating behaviour on the EAT-26 than participants with low and moderate use, however this finding was not statistically significant in relation to orthorexic behaviours. This research has implications for the mental health of young female TikTok users, with exposure to pro-anorexia content having immediate consequences for internalisation and body image dissatisfaction, potentially increasing one’s risk of developing disordered eating beliefs and behaviours.

Citation: Blackburn MR, Hogg RC (2024) #ForYou? the impact of pro-ana TikTok content on body image dissatisfaction and internalisation of societal beauty standards. PLoS ONE 19(8): e0307597. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597

Editor: Barbara Guidi, University of Pisa, ITALY

Received: November 2, 2023; Accepted: July 8, 2024; Published: August 7, 2024

Copyright: © 2024 Blackburn, Hogg. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: The data for this study can be found on Figshare via the following link: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25756800.v1 .

Funding: We acknowledge the financial support provided by Charles Sturt University.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Social media is a self-presentation device, a mode of entertainment, and a means of connecting with others [ 1 ], allowing for performance and the performance of identity [ 2 ], with social rewards built into its systems. Five to six years of the average human lifespan are now spent on social media sites [ 3 ] and visual platforms such as Instagram and TikTok increasingly dominate the cultural landscape of social media. Such visually oriented platforms are associated with higher levels of dysfunction in body image [ 4 ], while the COVID-19 pandemic has seen a rise in disordered eating behaviour [ 5 ]. Despite this, the field lacks a clear theoretical framework for understanding how social media usage heightens body image issues [ 6 ] and little research has specifically examined the impacts of TikTok based content. In this research, we sought to explore the impact of pro-anorexia TikTok content on body image satisfaction and internalisation of beauty standards for young women. The forthcoming sections of this literature review will highlight the features of social media content that may be particularly pernicious for young female users and will explore disordered eating and orthorexia in a social media context, concluding with a theoretical analysis of the relationship between social media and body image and internalisation of beauty standards, respectively.

Social media offers instant, quantifiable feedback coupled with idealised online imagery that may intersect with the value adolescents attribute to peer relationships and the sociocultural gender socialisation processes germane to this period of development, creating the “perfect storm” for young social media users, especially females [ 6 ]. In a study of 85 young, largely female eating disorder patients, a rise in awareness of online sites emphasizing thinness as beauty was evident from 2017 to 2020, with 60% of participants indicating that they knew of pro-ana websites and 22% of participants admitting to visiting them [ 7 ]. Research suggests that social media may also trigger those with extant eating disorders while simultaneously influencing healthy individuals to engage in disordered eating behaviour [ 8 ].

“Pro” eating disorder communities, hereafter referred to as “pro-ana” (pro-anorexia) communities, are a particular concern in a social media context. These communities encourage disordered eating, normalise disordered behaviours, and provide a means of connection for individuals who endorse anti-recovery from eating disorders [ 8 ]. Weight-loss tips, excessive exercise routines, and images of emaciated figures are routinely shared in these online communities [ 9 ], with extant research highlighting the association between viewing eating disorder content online and offline eating disorder behaviour [ 8 ]. Women who view pro-ana websites display increased eating disturbances, lowered body satisfaction, an increased drive for thinness, and higher levels of perfectionism when compared to women who have not viewed pro-ana content [ 10 , 11 ]. In research on adolescent girls, Stice [ 12 ] investigated the influence of exposure to media portraying the “thin-ideal” and found that perceived pressure to be thin was a predictor of increased body image dissatisfaction, which in turn led to increases in disordered eating behaviour. In similar research, Green [ 10 ] found that individuals with diagnosed eating disorders reported worsening symptoms after just 10-minutes of exposure to pro-ana content on the online platform, Tumblr.

Disordered eating #ForYou

The most downloaded social application (app) of 2021, TikTok is a social media platform that allows short-form video creation and sharing within a social media context [ 13 ]. Since its launch in 2017, TikTok has had over two billion downloads and has an estimated one billion users, the vast majority of which are children and teenagers [ 14 ]. Unlike other social media platforms where users have greater autonomy over the content generated on their homepage newsfeed, TikTok’s algorithm records data from single users and proposes videos designed to catch a user’s attention specifically, by creating a personalised “For You” page [ 15 ]. This feed will suggest videos from any creator on the platform, not just followed accounts. As such, if a user ‘interacts’ with a video, such as liking, sharing, commenting, or searching for related content, the algorithm will continue to produce similar videos on their “For You” page. The speed with which TikTok content can be created and consumed online may also be key to its impact. Any given social media user could watch more than a thousand videos on TikTok in an hour, creating a reinforcing effect that may have more impact than longer form content from a single creator [ 2 ].

Whilst the popularity of TikTok’s “For You” page has prompted global leaders in social media to build their own recommended content features, this feature remains most pronounced on TikTok. The “For You” page is the homepage of TikTok where users spend the majority of their time, compared to other social media platforms where homepages consist of a curation of content from followed accounts. Instagram’s explore page continues to emphasise established influencer culture and promote accounts of public figures or influencers with large followings. Contrastingly, TikTok’s unique algorithm makes content discoverability an even playing field, as any user’s content has the potential to reach a vast audience regardless of follower count or celebrity status. TikTok users therefore have less control over their homepage newsfeed compared to other social media platforms where users elect who they follow.

Unlike other social media platforms that implicitly showcase body ideals, TikTok contains explicit eating disorder content [ 16 ], while the “For You” page means that simply interacting with health and fitness videos can lead to unintended exposure to disordered eating content. Even seemingly benign “fitspiration” content may have psychological consequences for viewers. Beyond explicit pro-ana content, #GymTok and #FoodTok are two popular areas of content that provide a forum for users to create and consume content around their and others’ daily eating routines, weight loss transformations, and workout routines [ 2 ]. TikTok also frequently features content promoting clean eating, detox cleanses, and limited ingredient diets reflective of the current “food as medicine” movement of western culture [ 17 ], otherwise known as orthorexia. Despite efforts to ban such pro-ana related content, some videos easily circumvent controls [ 18 ], in part because many TikTok creators are non-public figures who are not liable to the backlash or cancellation that a public figure might receive for circulating socially irresponsible content.

Orthorexia: The rise of ‘healthy’ eating pathologies

Psychological analyses of eating disorders have historically focused on restrictive eating and the binge-purge cycle, however, more recently “positive” interests in nutrition have been examined. Orthorexia nervosa is characterised by a restrictive diet, ritualized patterns of eating, and rigid avoidance of foods deemed unhealthy or impure that consumes an individual’s focus [ 19 ]. Despite frequent observation of this distinct behavioural pattern by clinicians, orthorexia has received limited empirical attention and is not formally recognised as a psychiatric disorder [ 19 ]. Orthorexia and anorexia nervosa share traits of perfectionism, high trait anxiety, a high need to exert control, plus the potential for significant weight loss [ 19 ]. Termed ‘the disorder that cannot be diagnosed’ due to limited consensus around its features and the line between healthy and pathological eating practices, orthorexia mirrors the narrative of neoliberal self-improvement culture, wherein the body is treated as a site of performance and transformation.

Orthorexic restrictions and obsessions are routinely interpreted as signs of morality, health consciousness, and wellness [ 20 , 21 ]. Social media wellness influencers have played a significant role in normalising “clean [disordered] eating”. As one example, Turner and Lefevre [ 22 ] conducted an online survey of social media users following health food accounts and found that higher Instagram use was associated with a greater tendency towards orthorexia, with the prevalence of orthorexia among the study population at 49%, substantially higher than the general population (<1%). Similar health and food-related content on TikTok may provoke orthorexic tendencies among TikTok users, however, limited research has investigated orthorexic eating behaviour in the context of TikTok. The current study aims to bridge this gap in the literature around TikTok use and orthorexic tendencies. Disordered eating behaviour in the present study was measured by two separate but related constructs. ‘Restrictive’ disordered eating relates to dieting, oral control, and bulimic symptoms, whilst ‘healthy’ disordered eating constitutes orthorexic-like preoccupation with health food.

Theoretical analysis of body image and social media

An established risk factor in the development and maintenance of disordered eating behaviour is negative body image. Body image is a multidimensional construct that represents an individual’s perceptions and attitudes about their physical-self and encompasses an evaluative function through which individuals compare perceptions of their actual “self” to “ideal” images [ 23 ]. This comparison may produce feelings of dissatisfaction about one’s own body image if a significant discrepancy exists between the actual and ideal self-image [ 23 ]. Body image is not necessarily congruent with actual physique, with research demonstrating that women categorised as having a healthy body mass index (BMI) nonetheless report dissatisfaction with their weight and engage in restrictive dietary behaviours to reduce their weight [ 24 ]. In addition, body image dissatisfaction is considered normative in Western society, particularly among adolescent women [ 25 ]. This may be attributable to the constant flow of media that exposes women to unrealistic images of thinness idealized within society [ 26 ].

One theoretical framework for understanding social media’s relationship with body image is the Social Comparison Theory, proposed by Festinger [ 27 ] who suggests that people naturally evaluate themselves in comparison to others via upward or downward social comparisons. Research supports the notion that women who frequently engage in maladaptive upward appearance-related social comparisons are more likely to experience body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating [ 25 , 28 ], while visual exposure to thin bodies may detrimentally modulate one’s level of body image satisfaction [ 29 – 31 ]. In their study of undergraduate females, Engeln-Maddox [ 29 ] found that participants made upward social comparisons to images of thin models which were strongly associated with decreases in body image satisfaction and internalisation of thinness. Similarly, Tiggemann [ 32 ] found that adolescents who spent more time watching television featuring attractive actors and actresses reported an increased desire for thinness, theorised to be a result of increased social comparison to attractive media personalities.

The Transactional Model [ 33 ] extends Social Comparison Theory by emphasising the multifaceted and complex nature of social media influences on body image. This model acknowledges that individual differences may predispose a person to utilise social media for gratification, and highlights that as time spent on social media increases, so too does body image dissatisfaction [ 33 ]. In line with this, a recent review of literature by Frieiro Padín and colleagues [ 34 ] indicated that time spent on social media was strongly correlated with eating disorder psychopathologies, as well as heightened body image concerns, internalisation of the thin ideal, and lower levels of self-esteem. Time on social media also correlated with heightened body image concerns to a far greater extent than general internet usage [ 35 , 36 ].

Body image ideals are not static. The traditional ideal of rib-protruding bodies from the 90s, known colloquially as “heroin chic”, have recently shifted to a celebration of the “slim-thicc” figure, consisting of a cinched, flat waist with curvy hips, ample breasts, and large behinds [ 37 ]. The “slim-thicc” aesthetic allows women to be bigger than previous body ideals, yet this figure is arguably more unattainable than the thin-ideal as surgical intervention is commonly needed to achieve it, depending on genetics and body type. The idealisation of the “slim-thicc” figure is highlighted by the “Brazilian butt lift” (BBL), a potentially life-threatening procedure that is nonetheless the fastest growing category of plastic surgery, doubling in growth over the past five years, despite the life-threatening potential of the procedure [ 38 ]. Research suggests that the slim-thicc ideal is no less damaging nor threatening of body image than the thin-ideal. Indeed, in experimental research on body ideals, McComb and Mills [ 39 ] found that the greatest body dissatisfaction levels in female undergraduate students were observed among those exposed to imagery of the slim-thicc physique, relative to that exhibited by those exposed to the thin-ideal and fit-ideal physique, as well as the control condition.

Recent body ideals have also favoured muscular thin presentations, considered to represent health and fitness as evident in the “#fitspiration” Instagram hashtag that features over 65 million images [ 40 ]. Fitspiration has the potential to positively influence women’s health and wellbeing by promoting exercise engagement and healthy eating, yet various content analyses of fitspiration images highlight aspects of fitspiration that warrant concern [see 40 , 41 ]. Notably, fitspiration typically showcases only one body type and women whose bodies do not meet this standard may experience body dissatisfaction [ 40 ], while the gamification of exercise, such as receiving likes for every ten sit-ups, segues with the intensive self-control and competitiveness that often underpins eating disorders and eating disorder communities [ 1 ].

In recent experimental research, Pryde and Prichard [ 42 ] examined the effect of exposure to fitspiration TikTok content on the body dissatisfaction, appearance comparison, and mood of young Australian women. Viewing fitspiration TikTok videos led to increased negative mood and increased appearance comparison but did not impact body dissatisfaction. This finding contradicts previous research and may be due to fitspiration content showcasing body functionality rather than aesthetic, which may lead to positive outcomes for viewers. The fitspiration content used by Pryde and Prichard [ 42 ] did not contain the harmful themes regularly found in other forms of fitspiration content. Appearance comparison was significant in the relationship between TikTok content and body dissatisfaction and mood, suggesting that this may be a key mechanism through which fitspiration content leads to negative body image outcomes and supporting the notion that fitspiration promotes a focus on appearance rather than health.

Body image dissatisfaction among women is associated with co-morbid psychological disturbances and the development of disordered eating behaviours [ 43 , 44 ]. A large body of research indicates that higher levels of both general and appearance-related social comparison are associated with disordered eating in undergraduate populations [ 10 , 28 , 45 – 48 ]. As one example, Lindner et al. [ 46 ] investigated the impact of the female-to-male ratio of college campuses on female students’ engagement in social comparison and eating pathology. Their findings lend support to the Social Comparison Theory, indicating that the highest levels of eating pathology and social comparison were found among women attending colleges with predominantly female undergraduate populations. A strong relationship was also found between eating pathology and engagement in appearance-related social comparisons independent of actual weight. Lindner et al. [ 46 ] surmised that these results suggest social comparison and eating pathology behaviours are due to students’ perceptual distortions of their own bodies, potentially fostered by pressures exerted from peers to be thin.

Similarly, Corning et al. [ 45 ] investigated the social comparison behaviours of women with eating disorder symptoms and their asymptomatic peers. Results illustrated that a greater tendency to engage in everyday social comparison predicted the presence of eating disorder symptoms, while women with eating disorder symptoms made significantly more social comparisons of their own bodies. Such findings are supported by subsequent research, with Hamel et al. [ 28 ] finding that adolescents with a diagnosed eating disorder engaged in significantly more body-related social comparison than adolescents diagnosed with a depressive disorder or no diagnosis. Body-related social comparison was also significantly positively correlated with disordered eating behaviours. While extant research has focused upon social comparison as it has occurred through traditional media outlets, less research has investigated the facilitation of social comparison through social media platforms, particularly contemporary platforms such as TikTok.

Theoretical analysis of internalisation processes and social media

The extent to which one’s body image is impacted by images and messages conveyed by the media is determined by the degree to which these images and messages are internalised. Some may argue that social media platforms are distinct from what occurs in “real” life, creating fewer opportunities for internalisation to occur. Yet as Pierce [ 2 ] argues, platforms such as TikTok create their own realities, allowing users to explore their identities, form relationships, engage with culture and world events, and even develop new patterns of speech and writing. TikTok trends commonly infiltrate society, underscoring the impact of social media on life beyond the online world and thus a sociocultural analysis of TikTok is warranted. Sociocultural theories suggest that society portrays thinness as the ideal body shape for women, resulting in an internationalisation of the “thin is good” assumption for women. This in turn results in lowered body image satisfaction and other negative outcomes [ 43 ]. The significance of social influences, including the role of family, peers, and the media, is emphasised by sociocultural theory, with individuals more likely to internalise the thin ideal when they encounter pressuring messages that they are not thin enough from social influences [ 48 ]. Within this theoretical approach, an individual’s degree of thin ideal internalisation is theorised to depend on their acceptance of socially defined ideals of attractiveness and is reflected in their engagement in behaviours that adhere to these socially defined ideals [ 49 ].

Building on this, the tripartite influence model suggests that disordered eating behaviours arise due to pressure from social agents, specifically media, family, and peers. This pressure centres on conforming to appearance ideals and may lead to engagement in social comparison and the internalisation of thin ideals [ 48 ]. This is relevant in a digital context given social media provides endless opportunities for individuals to practice social comparison and for many users, social comparison on TikTok is peer-based as well as media-based. According to the tripartite model, social comparisons have been consistently associated with a higher degree of thin ideal internalisation, self-objectification, drive for thinness, and weight dissatisfaction [ 50 ]. Furthermore, and in contrast to traditional media where social agents are mainly models, celebrities, and movie stars, social agents on social media can include peers, friends, family, and individuals who have a relationship with the individual. Social media content generated by “everyday” people, rather than super models or movie stars, may result in comparisons that are more horizontal in nature. This is particularly evident on TikTok where content creators are rarely famous before creating a TikTok account, often remain micro-influencers after achieving some notoriety, and are usually around the same age as those viewing their content.

Pressure to be thin from alike peers may have a particularly pronounced impact on one’s degree of internalisation of the thinness ideal. Indeed, Stice et al. [ 51 ] found that after listening to young thin women complain about “feeling fat”, their adolescent participant sample reported increased body image dissatisfaction, suggesting that pressure from peers perpetuates the thinness ideal, leading to internalisation of the ideal and subsequent body dissatisfaction. Similarly, it was found that adolescent females were more likely to engage in weight loss behaviour if a high portion of peers with a similar BMI were also engaging in these behaviours, illustrating that appearance pressure exerted by alike peers may result in thin-ideal internalisation and engagement in weight loss behaviours to control body weight and shape [ 52 ]. Such findings raise questions around whether those most similar to us have the greatest impact upon thin-ideal internalisation, body image dissatisfaction, and disordered eating behaviours.

In further support for the tripartite influence model, research by Thompson et al. [ 48 ] indicates that the ideals promoted through social media trends are internalized despite being unattainable, resulting in body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating behaviour. Similarly, Mingoia et al. [ 53 ] found a positive association between the use of social networking sites and thin ideal internalisation in women, indicating that greater use of social networking sites was linked to significantly higher internalisation of the thin ideal. Interestingly, the use of appearance-related features (e.g., posting or viewing photographs or videos) was more strongly related with internalisation than the broad use of social networking sites (e.g., writing status’, messaging features) [ 53 ]. Correlational and experimental research alike has demonstrated that thin ideal internalisation is related to body image dissatisfaction and leads to expressions of disordered eating such as restrictive dieting and binge-purge symptoms [ 31 , 48 , 54 , 55 ]. Subsequent expressions of disordered eating may be seen as an attempt to control weight and body shape to conform to societal beauty standards of thinness [ 51 ].

This sociocultural perspective is exemplified by Grabe et al’s. [ 54 ] meta-analysis of research on the associations between media exposure to women’s body dissatisfaction, internalisation of the thin ideal, and eating behaviours and beliefs, illustrating that exposure to media images propagating the thin ideal is related to and indeed, may lead to body image concerns and increased endorsement of disordered eating behaviours in women. Similarly, Groesz et al. [ 55 ] conducted a meta-analysis to examine experimental manipulations of the thin beauty ideal. They found that body image was significantly more negative after viewing thin media images than after viewing images of average size models, plus size models, or inanimate objects. This effect size was stronger for participants who were more vulnerable to activation of the thinness schema. Groesz et al. [ 55 ] conclude that their results align with the sociocultural theory perspective that media promulgates a thin ideal that in turn provokes body dissatisfaction.

Current research

Existing research has established the relationship between body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating behaviours and social media platforms such as Instagram and Twitter. The unique implications of the TikTok ‘For You Page’, as well as the dominance of peer-created and explicit disordered eating content on TikTok suggests that the influence of this platform warrants specific consideration. This study adds to extant literature by utilising an experimental design to examine the influence of exposure to pro-ana TikTok content on body image and internalisation of societal beauty standards. A cross-sectional design was used to investigate the effect of daily TikTok and the development of disordered eating behaviours. Although body image disturbance and eating disorders are not limited to women, varying sociocultural factors have been implicated in the development of disordered eating behaviour in men and women [ 45 ], while issues facing trans people warrant specific consideration beyond the scope of this study, therefore the present sample contains only female-identifying participants.

Aims and hypotheses.

The current study aimed to investigate the impact of pro-ana TikTok content on young women’s body image satisfaction and internalisation of beauty standards, as well as exploring daily TikTok use and the development of disordered eating behaviour. First, in line with the cross-sectional component of the study, it was hypothesized that women who spend greater time on TikTok per day would report significantly more disordered eating behaviour than women who spend low amounts of time on TikTok per day. Second, it was hypothesized that women in the pro-ana TikTok group would report a significant decrease in body image satisfaction state following exposure to the pro-ana content compared to women in the control group. Third, it was hypothesized that women in the pro-ana Tik Tok group would report increased internalisation of societal beauty standards following exposure to pro-ana TikTok content compared to women in the control group.

Participants

Participants in the current study included 273 women aged between 18 to 28 years sourced from the general population of TikTok users. The predominant country of residence of the sample was Australia, with 15 participants indicating they currently reside outside of Australia. Of the remaining data relating to the two conditions of the study, 126 participants were randomly allocated into the experimental condition, and 147 participants were randomly allocated into the control condition. Snowball sampling was used to recruit participants through social media, online survey sharing platforms, and word-of-mouth, with first-year University students targeted for recruitment by offering class credit in return for participation. Participants could withdraw their consent at any time by exiting the study prior to completion of the survey.

The current study employed a questionnaire set that included a demographic questionnaire, and five scales measuring disordered eating behaviour, body satisfaction, and internalisation of societal beauty standards, as well as perfectionism, the latter of which was not examined in the present study.

Demographic questionnaire.

The demographic questionnaire required participants to answer a series of questions relating to their gender, age, relationship status, ethnicity, country of residence, TikTok usage, and exercise routine. A screening question redirected non-female-identifying persons from the study. Responses to the TikTok usage items were examined cross-sectionally with responses on the EAT-26 and ORTO15 used to examine the influence of daily TikTok use and the presentation of disordered eating behaviours.

Eating attitudes test.

The Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26, [ 56 ]) is a short form of the original 40-item EAT scale [ 57 ] which measures symptoms and concerns characteristic of eating disorders. The 26-item short-form version of the EAT was utilised in the present study due to its established reliability and validity, and strong correlation with the EAT-40 [ 56 ].

Responses to the 26-items are self-reported using a 6-point Likert scale ranging from Always (3) to Never (0) [ 56 ]. The EAT-26 consists of three subscales including dieting, bulimia and food preoccupation, and oral control. Five behavioural questions are included in Part C of the EAT-26 to determine the presence and frequency of extreme weight-control behaviours including binge eating, self-induced vomiting, laxative usage, and excessive exercise [ 56 ]. Higher scores indicate greater disordered eating behaviour, and those with a total score of 20 or greater are, in clinical contexts, typically highlighted as requiring further assessment and advice of a mental health professional [ 56 ].

Internal consistency of the EAT-26 was established in initial psychometric studies which reported a Cronbach’s alpha of.85 [ 58 ]. For the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha = .91. Previous research has also demonstrated that the EAT-26 has strong test-retest reliability (e.g., 0.84) [ 59 ], as well as acceptable criterion-related validity for differentiating between eating disorder populations and non-disordered populations [ 56 ]. In the current study, the EAT-26 was used to measure disordered eating behaviour, and the cut-off score of 20 and above was adopted to categorise increased disordered eating behaviour. Given how this construct is measured, from this point forward the present study will refer to EAT-26 responses as ‘restrictive’ type disordered eating.

The ORTO-15 is a 15-item screening measure that assesses orthorexia nervosa risk through questions regarding the perceived effects of eating healthy food (e.g. “Do you think that consuming healthy food may improve your appearance?”), eating habits (e.g. “At present, are you alone when having meals?”), and the extent to which concerns about food influence daily life (e.g. “Does the thought of food worry you for more than three hours a day?”) [ 19 ]. Responses are self-reported using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from always , often , sometimes , or never . Individual items are coded and summed to derive a total score. Donini et al. [ 60 ] established a cut off total score of 40; scores below 40 indicate orthorexia behaviours, whilst scores 40 or above reflect normal eating behaviour. This cut off score was determined by Donini et al. [ 60 ] as their results revealed the ORTO-15 demonstrated good predictive capability at the threshold of 40 compared to other potential threshold values.

Although the ORTO-15 is the most widely accepted screening tool to assess orthorexia risk, it is still only partially validated [ 61 ], and inconsistencies of the measures’ reliability and validity exist in current literature. For example, Roncero et al. [ 62 ] estimated that the reliability of the ORTO-15 using Cronbach’s alpha was between 0.20 and 0.23, however, after removing certain items, the reliability coefficients were between 0.74 and 0.83. Contrastingly, Costa and colleagues’ [ 63 ] review of current literature surrounding orthorexia suggested adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83 to 0.91) with all 15-items.

In the present study, a reliability analysis revealed unacceptable reliability for the ORTO-15 (α = .24). Principal components factor analysis identified two factors within the ORTO-15, one relating to dieting and the other to preoccupation with health food. Separate reliability analyses were performed on the items that comprised these two factors and the diet-related items did not have acceptable reliability (α = -.40), whilst the health food-related items bordered on acceptable reliability at α = .63. Consequently, only the health food-related items were retained in the current study following consideration of Pallant’s [ 64 ] assertion that Cronbach alpha values are sensitive to the number of items on a scale and it is therefore common to obtain low values on scales with less than ten items. Pallant [ 64 ] notes that in cases such as this, it is appropriate to report the inter-item correlation of the items, while Briggs and Cheek [ 65 ] advise an optimal range for the inter-item correlation between.2 to.4, with the health food-related items in the current study obtaining an inter-item correlation of.25. Throughout this study, the construct measured by these ORTO-15 items will be referred to as ‘healthy’ type disordered eating to reflect this obsessive health food preoccupation and differentiate between the two disordered eating dependent variables measured in the current study.

Body image states scale.

The Body Image States Scale (BISS) by Cash and colleagues [ 66 ] is a six-item measure of momentary evaluative and affective experiences of one’s own physical appearance. The BISS evaluates the following aspects of current body experience: dissatisfaction-satisfaction with overall physical appearance; dissatisfaction-satisfaction with one’s body size and shape; dissatisfaction-satisfaction with one’s weight; feelings of physical attractiveness-unattractiveness; current feelings about one’s looks relative to how one usually feels; and evaluation of one’s appearance relative to how the average person looks [ 66 ]. Participants responded to these items using a 9-point Likert-type scale which is presented in a negative-to-positive direction for half of the items, and a positive-to-negative direction for the other half [ 66 ]. Respondents were instructed to select the statement that best captured how they felt “ right now at this very moment ”. A total BISS score was calculated by reverse-scoring the three positive-to-negative items, summing the six-items, and finding the mean, with higher total BISS scores indicating more favourable body image states.

During the development and implementation of the BISS, Cash and colleagues [ 66 ] report acceptable internal consistency and moderate stability over time, an anticipated outcome due to the nature of the BISS as a state assessment tool. The BISS was also appropriately correlated with a range of trait measures of body image, highlighting its convergent validity [ 66 ]. Cash and colleagues [ 66 ] also report that the BISS is sensitive to reactions in positive and negative situational contexts and has good construct validity. An acceptable Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of.88 was obtained in the current study.

Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire—4.

The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire– 4 (SATAQ-4) [ 67 ] is a 22-item self-report questionnaire that assesses the influence of interpersonal and sociocultural appearance ideals on one’s body image, eating disturbance, and self-esteem. Ratings are captured on a 5-point Likert scale which asks participants to specify their level of agreement with each statement by choosing from 1 ( definitely disagree ) through to 5 ( definitely agree ), with higher scores indicative of greater pressure to conform to, or greater internalisation of, interpersonal and sociocultural appearance ideals [ 67 ]. The five subscales of the SATAQ-4 measure: internalisation of thin/low body fat ideals, internalisation of muscular/athletic ideals, influence of pressures from family, influence of pressure from peers, and influence of pressures from the media [ 67 ]. For the purposes of the present study, the questions from the media pressure subscale were modified to enquire specifically about social media rather than traditional forms of media.

Across all samples in Schaefer et al’s. [ 67 ] study, the internal consistency of the five SATAQ-4 subscales is considered acceptable to excellent, with Cronbach’s alpha scores between 0.75 and 0.95. These subscales also displayed good convergent validity with other measures of body satisfaction, eating disorder risk, and self-esteem [ 67 ]. Pearson product-moment correlations between the SATAQ-4 subscales and convergent measures revealed medium to large positive associations with eating disorder symptomology, medium negative associations with body satisfaction, and small negative associations with self-esteem [ 67 ]. A Cronbach’s alpha of.87 was obtained in the present study, demonstrating acceptable internal consistency.

Ethical approval for the present study was granted by the Charles Sturt University Human Research Ethics Committee (Approval number H21155) prior to data collection. Participants were directed to the study via an online link to QuestionPro where they were provided an explanation of the study, their rights, and contact details of relevant support services if they were to become distressed. Participants gave informed consent by clicking on a link that read, “I consent to participate” at the beginning of the survey and then again through the submission of their completed survey. Any incomplete responses were not included in the dataset. Data collection commenced on the 30 th of July 2021 and ceased on the 1st of October 2021. In line with the cross-sectional and descriptive aspects of the research, participants were asked demographic questions about their gender, age, relationship status, ethnicity, country of residence, TikTok usage, and exercise habits. Participants then completed the experimental set in the following order: BISS (pre-test), SATAQ-4 (pre-test), EAT-26, ORTO-15, Experimental intervention (control or experimental TikTok video condition), SATAQ-4 (post-test), BISS (post-test), and debrief. All questionnaires presented to each participant were identical. Measures were not randomised to ensure that body image and internalisation were assessed at both pre- and post-test to evaluate the experimental manipulation.

Participants were randomly allocated to one of two conditions: experimental (pro-ana TikTok video) or control (“normal” TikTok video). Participants allocated to the experimental condition watched a compilation of TikTok videos containing explicit disordered eating messages such as young women restricting their food, displaying gallows humour about their disordered eating behaviour, starving themselves, and providing weight loss tips such as eating ice cubes and chewing gum to curve hunger. Participants in the experimental condition were also exposed to more implicit body image ideals typical of fitspiration-style content. This included thin women displaying their abdomens, cinched waists, dancing in two-piece swimwear, along with workout and juice cleanse videos promising fast weight loss. Participants in the control condition viewed a compilation of TikTok videos containing scenes relating to nature, cooking and recipes, animals, and comedy. After viewing the 7- to 8-minute TikTok video, all participants completed measures of internalisation and body satisfaction again to assess the influence of either the pro-ana TikTok video or the normal TikTok video. The debrief statement made explicit to participants the rationale of the study and explained the non-normative content of the videos shown to the experimental group. A small financial incentive was offered via a prize draw of five vouchers.

Statistical analysis

The data from QuestionPro was collated and analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics software, Version 28. All measures and manipulations in the study have been disclosed, alongside the method of determining the final sample size. No data collection was conducted following analysis of the data. Data for this study is available via the Figshare data repository and can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25756800.v1 . This study was not preregistered. Sample size was determined before any data analysis. A priori power analyses were conducted using G*Power to determine the minimum sample sizes required to test the study hypotheses. Results indicated the required sample sizes to achieve 90% power for detecting medium effects, with a significance criterion of α = 0.05, were: N = 108 for the mixed between-within subjects ANOVAs and N = 232 for the one-way between groups ANOVAs. According to these recommendations, adequate statistical power was achieved. All univariate and multivariate assumptions were checked and found to be met. All scales and independent variables were normally distributed.

The analysis of the current study including data screening processes, descriptive statistics, and hypothesis testing will be presented in this section. Hypothesis testing began with two separate mixed between-within subjects analysis of variance models (ANOVAs) to examine the impact of the experimental manipulation on the independent variables of body image and internalisation of appearance ideals and pressure. Finally, the effect of time spent using TikTok daily on restrictive and ‘healthy’ disordered eating behaviour was explored cross-sectionally using two separate one-way between-subjects ANOVAs.

Data screening

Prior to statistical analysis, data were screened for entry errors and missing data. Of the 838 participants who initially consented to participate in the survey, 555 responses were insufficiently complete for data analysis. As participants were permitted to withdraw their consent by exiting the online survey, these results were excluded from all subsequent analyses. Of those that did not complete the study, the majority withdrew during the BISS (pre-test) and the ORTO-15, suggesting that these participants potentially experienced discomfort or distress when asked to reflect on their appearance and their eating behaviours. Of the completed responses, nine were excluded due to not meeting the study’s stated age eligibility and another case was excluded due to disclosure of a previous eating disorder diagnosis. The remaining data set comprised of 273 participants.

Descriptive statistics

Demographic characteristics..

In the current sample, 50% of participants reported being currently single and most participants (83%) were Caucasian, with 71% of participants indicating that they spent up to two hours per day using TikTok. Further demographic information is provided in Table 1 .

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.t001

#ForYou: TikTok consumption demographics.

Participants in the current study reported that entertainment (75%), fashion (59%), beauty/skincare (54%), cooking/recipes (51%) and life hacks/advice (51%) content frequently occurred on their For You page. Largely in keeping with this, participants reported experiencing the most enjoyment from viewing entertainment (84%), life hacks/advice (57%), home renovation (56%), recipes/cooking (56%), and fashion (54%) content on their For You page.

In the current sample, 64% of participants reported being exposed to disordered eating content via their For You page. Only 15% of participants had not been exposed to any negative content themes. Further descriptive For You page content information is displayed below in Table 2 .

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.t002

Notably, 43% of the participant sample were frequently exposed to fitness and sports related content and the same percentage of the sample enjoyed seeing this content, suggesting that content broadly aligned with #fitspiration was consumed and appreciated by nearly half of participants. Concerningly, 40–60% of participants had been exposed to negative TikTok content via the For You Page, with content ranging from self-harm and suicidality to violence and illegal activity. No data was collected on the specifics of this content, however, and it is possible that some “negative” content may be framed from a proactive, preventative perspective, and this warrants further consideration.

Hypothesis testing: Cross-sectional analysis

Hypothesis 1: daily tiktok use and disordered eating behaviour..

To test the cross-sectional analysis of this study, two separate one-way between-groups ANOVAs were conducted to explore the impact of daily amount of TikTok use on ‘healthy’ disordered eating and restrictive disordered eating behaviour. This was necessary as time on TikTok was measured categorically. Participants were divided into four groups according to their average daily time spent using TikTok (Low use group: 1 hour or less; Moderate use group: 1–2 hours; High use group: 2–3 hours; Extreme use group: 3+ hours). Homogeneity of variance could be assumed for each ANOVA as indicated by non-significant Levene’s Test Statistics.

There was no statistically significant difference at the p < .05 level in ORTO15 scores for the four TikTok usage groups: F (3, 269) = .38, p = .78, indicating that ‘healthy’ disordered eating did not significantly differ across women who use TikTok for different periods of time per day. The effect size, calculated using eta squared, was.004, which is considered small in Cohen’s [ 68 ] terms. This small effect size is congruent with the non-significant finding.

The second ANOVA measuring differences among EAT-26 scores across the four TikTok usage groups also yielded a non-significant result: F (3, 269) = 1.21, p = .31. Eta squared was calculated as.01, representing a small effect size [ 68 ] consistent with this non-significant result. The means and standard deviations of the four TikTok usage groups across dependent variables of ‘healthy’ and restrictive disordered eating, as measured by the ORTO15 and the EAT-26 respectively, are displayed in Table 3 .

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.t003

Hypothesis testing: Experimental analyses

Hypothesis 2: body image satisfaction across groups from pre-test to post-test..

To evaluate the effect of the experimental intervention on body image, a 2 x 2 mixed between-within subjects ANOVA was conducted with condition (experimental vs control) as the between subjects factor and time (pre-manipulation vs post-manipulation) as the within subjects factor. All assumptions were upheld, including homogeneity of variance-covariance as indicated by Box’s M ( p >.001) and Levene’s ( p >.05) tests [ 64 ].

The interaction between condition and time was significant, Wilks’ Lambda = .98, F (1, 271) = 6.83, p = .009, partial eta squared = .03, demonstrating that the change in body image scores from pre-manipulation to post-manipulation was significantly different for the two groups. The body image satisfaction scores for women in both conditions decreased from pre-manipulation to post-manipulation. As anticipated, participants in the experimental condition reported a greater decrease in body image satisfaction than women in the control condition (see Table 4 ). This interaction effect is displayed in Fig 1 .

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.g001

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.t004

Although not consequential to the testing of the experimental manipulation, statistically significant main effects were also found for time, Wilks’ Lambda = .89, F (1, 271) = 32.99, p = < .001, partial eta squared = .109 and condition, F (1, 271) = 4.42, p = .036, partial eta squared = .016. The means and standard deviations of these main effects are displayed in Table 4 .

Hypothesis 3: Internalisation of societal beauty standards across groups from pre-test to post-test.

A second 2 x 2 mixed between-within subjects ANOVA was conducted to investigate the effect of the experimental manipulation on participants’ internalisation scores. All assumptions for the mixed model ANOVA were met with no violations.

A statistically significant interaction was found between group condition and time, Wilks’ Lambda = .97, F (1, 271) = 8.16, p = .005, partial eta squared = .029. This significant interaction highlights that the change in degree of internalisation at pre-manipulation and post-manipulation is not the same for the two conditions. Interestingly, the internalisation scores for women in the control group decreased from pre-manipulation to post-manipulation, whilst as anticipated, internalisation scores for women in the experimental group increased following exposure to the manipulation (see Table 5 ). This interaction is displayed in Fig 2 .

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.g002

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0307597.t005

No statistically significant main effects were found for time, Wilks’ Lambda = .987, F (1, 271) = 3.59, p = .059, partial eta squared = .013 or condition, F (1, 271) = 2.65, p = .104, partial eta squared = .010. The means and standard deviations of internalisation scores for each condition at pre-manipulation and post-manipulation are displayed below in Table 5 .

The current study investigated the effect of TikTok content on women’s body image satisfaction and degree of internalisation of appearance ideals, and whether greater TikTok use contributed to increased disordered eating behaviour. In support of the hypotheses, exposure to pro-ana TikTok content significantly decreased participants’ body image satisfaction and increased participants’ degree of internalisation of appearance ideals. The hypothesis that greater daily TikTok use would contribute to increased disordered eating behaviour was not supported, as no statistically significant differences in restrictive disordered eating or ‘healthy’ disordered eating were found between the low, moderate, high, and extreme daily TikTok use groups.

Cross-sectional findings

Daily tiktok use and disordered eating behaviour..

Contrary to expectations, differences among groups on measures of restrictive disordered eating and ‘healthy’ disordered eating did not reach statistical significance. The proposed hypothesis that greater daily TikTok usage would be associated with disordered eating behaviour and attitudes was therefore unsupported. Despite lacking statistical support, participants categorised in the ‘high’ and ‘extreme’ daily TikTok use groups reported an average EAT-26 score of 18.16 and 19.09, respectively. Considering that an EAT-26 cut-off of ≥ 20 indicates potential clinical psychopathology, this mean score illustrates that exposure to TikTok content for two or more hours per day may contribute to a clinical degree of restrictive disordered eating.

The failure of the present study to detect any significant differences in disordered eating behaviours among participants with different TikTok daily usage does not align with the Transactional Model [ 33 ]. According to this model, risk factors such as low self-esteem and high thin ideal internalisation may predispose an individual to seek gratification via social media, resulting in body dissatisfaction and negative affect. The Transactional Model therefore proposes that a positive correlation exists between time spent on social media and body image dissatisfaction. Our findings also do not align with the conclusions Frieiro Padín et al. [ 34 ] drew from their review of the literature, in which a strong connection was identified between time on social media and heightened body image concerns and internalisation of the thin ideal, as well as eating disorder psychopathologies, though a distinction in outcome measures must be noted.

Based on the aforementioned sociocultural theory and previous research [see 28 , 43 , 48 ], it was assumed that increased body dissatisfaction as a result of increased time spent on social media (as stipulated by the Transactional Model), would lead to greater disordered eating behaviour. However, this was not supported statistically in the data. As postulated by Culbert et al. [ 69 ], disordered eating behaviour may instead only be a risk of media exposure if individuals are prone to endorse thin-ideals. Individuals in the present study that reported ‘high’ and ‘extreme’ daily TikTok use may have felt satisfied with their bodies and experienced lower thin-ideal internalisation. This could have potentially buffered the negative effect of greater TikTok content exposure and accounted for the lack of significant differences in disordered eating behaviour between groups. The quantity of TikTok consumption remains a pertinent question for disordered eating behaviour. As per the present study’s brief experimental manipulation, findings suggest that high frequency of daily TikTok use does not necessarily contribute to greater disordered eating behaviour than short exposures to this content.

Content presented to the pro-ana TikTok group included a mix of explicit and implicit pro- eating disorder messages as well as fitspiration content. Fitspiration content presented in the current study included workout videos to achieve a “smaller waist” and “toned abs” where female creators with slim, toned physiques sporting activewear took viewers through a series of exercises, advising viewers that they would “see results in a week”. In the present study, diet-related fitspiration content presented included the concoction of juices to “get rid of belly fat” and advice on the best “diet for a small waist” which requires avoidance of all meat, dairy, junk food, soda, and above all, to make “no excuses”. Fitspiration style content in the current study totalled one-minute, compared to disordered eating themes which totalled six minutes. The integration of these various types of content, although reflective of the For You function in TikTok, impeded our ability to determine the singular impact of fitspiration or disordered eating content, respectively, on body image and internalisation of societal beauty standards, but did reflect social media as it is consumed beyond experimental research settings.

Experimental findings

Tiktok and body image states..

The hypothesis that women exposed to pro-ana TikTok content would experience a significant decrease in body image compared to women who viewed the control TikTok content was supported. The present study found a significant interaction effect of body image between group condition (control vs experimental) and time period (pre-manipulation vs post-manipulation), as well as significant main effects. It is important to note that the statistic of interest in evaluating the success of the experimental manipulation is the interaction effect, thus main effects must be interpreted secondarily and with caution [ 64 ]. Women in the experimental group reported significantly lower body image satisfaction after exposure to the pro-ana TikTok content and compared to women who viewed the control content. This finding corroborates Festinger’s [ 27 ] Social Comparison Theory that posits people naturally evaluate themselves in comparison to others. Exposure to the pro-ana TikTok content, consisting of various thin bodies and messaging around weight loss, may have provided the opportunity for women to engage in maladaptive upward social comparisons, resulting in reduced body image satisfaction. The present study upholds previous findings of Engeln-Maddox, Tiggemann, McComb and Mills, and Gibson [ 29 , 32 , 39 , 70 ] who suggest that visual exposure to thin bodies may adversely affect one’s level of body image satisfaction and extends this research by replicating this finding in the context of a contemporary media platform, TikTok, and by utilising an experimental design.

Contradicting the present study and previous research, Pryde and Prichard [ 42 ] found no significant increase in young women’s body dissatisfaction following exposure to fitspiration TikTok content. A potential explanation for this finding is that the performance of physical movements captured in fitspiration videos may shift the focus of viewers from aesthetics to functionality, highlighting physical competencies and capabilities which has been shown to improve body image satisfaction in young women [ 71 ]. Pryde and Prichard’s [ 42 ] fitspiration content did not include typically occurring harmful themes as the present study did, potentially reducing the negative implications for body image satisfaction of exposure to such content in real world contexts.

Interestingly, women in the control group also reported a statistically significant decrease in body image satisfaction after viewing the neutral TikTok content, a finding that underscores the possible complexity of social media’s influence on body image, as identified in research by Huülsing [ 72 ]. This is an unexpected finding, as the TikTok content displayed to the control group was selected specifically to be unrelated to appearance ideals and pressures. One possible reason for this result is the repetition of administration of the BISS within a short time period. Completing the BISS twice may have caused participants to focus more attention on their body appearance than usual, resulting in more critical appraisals regardless of the experimental stimuli to which they were exposed. This notion aligns with previous research that found focusing on the appearance of body was associated with lower body image satisfaction, whereas focus on the function of the body was associated with more positive body image states [ 71 ].

One potential explanation for this finding is that the control group stimuli was contaminated and produced an unintentional effect on body image scores. Two-minutes of footage within the seven-minute control group TikTok compilation presented the human body including legs, arms, and hands. Although this body-related content was neutral in nature, it may be that even ‘harmless’ representations of the human body are sufficient to elicit a social comparison response in participants or in some capacity, reinforce the #fitspiration motifs commonly depicted on TikTok [ 1 ], therefore impacting body image scores at post-manipulation. This possible explanation has implications for TikTok use and women’s body image, as it suggests that viewing even benign content of human bodies for less than 10-minutes can have an immediate detrimental impact on body image states, even when this content is unrelated to body dissatisfaction, thinness, or weight loss. Furthermore, although a statistically significant body image decrease was detected in the control group, this finding must be interpreted with caution due to the significant interaction effect obtained.

TikTok and internalisation of societal beauty standards.

In accordance with the hypothesis, women in the experimental group reported a significant increase in their degree of internalisation of appearance ideals following exposure to pro-ana TikTok content. Women in the experimental group also reported significantly greater internalisation of appearance ideals than women in the control group. Conversely to the experimental group, internalisation scores of the control group decreased after viewing the neutral TikTok content. These findings are in line with the sociocultural theory, as women reported increased internalisation of societal beauty standards following exposure to media content explicitly and implicitly portraying the thinness ideal. The present study supports Mingoia et al’s. [ 53 ] meta-analysis, which yielded a positive association between social networking site use and the extent of internalisation of the thin ideal and furthers this notion by replicating the finding with TikTok specifically and utilising an experimental design.

In the current study, participants were subject to a single brief exposure of pro-ana TikTok content, whereas most of the sample indicated that their TikTok use was up to two hours per day. This suggests that the degree of internalisation of appearance ideals in participants lives outside of the experiment are likely to be much greater. Mingoia et al. [ 53 ] also found that the use of appearance-related features on social networking sites, such as posting and viewing photos and videos, demonstrated a stronger relationship with the internalisation of the thin ideal than the use of social networking features that were not appearance-related, such as messaging and writing status updates. As TikTok is a video sharing app and most of its content generally features full-body-length camera shots rather than a face or head shot, this finding suggests that TikTok users could potentially internalise body-related societal standards to a greater extent than users of other social media apps that typically feature head shots.

The finding that women internalised societal beauty standards to a greater degree after being exposed to pro-ana TikTok content corroborates the sociocultural theory’s emphasis of the significance of social influences in internalisation. TikTok users may be exposed to all three social influences (i.e., media, peers, and family) simultaneously on a single platform which may encourage internalisation of appearance-ideals in a more profound manner than any of these three influences in isolation. One point of difference between TikTok and other social media apps is that much content on the app is generated by “ordinary” individuals, rather than supermodels or celebrities. This enables blatantly insidious and diet-related content to circulate the app with less policing and scrutiny compared to content produced by an influencer or celebrity who may be more likely to be criticised or cancelled for socially irresponsible messaging and also provides the opportunity for more horizontal social comparisons and peer-to-peer style interactions rather than upward social comparisons.

Indeed, in their study of American teens, Mueller et al. [ 52 ] identified that girls were especially likely to engage in weight loss behaviour if a high proportion of girls with a similar BMI were also engaging in weight loss behaviours. This indicates that internalisation was strongest when appearance-ideals were promoted by alike peers. Due to the fact that much pro-ana TikTok content is created by young women, Mueller et al’s. [ 52 ] finding has problematic implications for the young female users of TikTok, in that harmful diet-related messages could be internalised to a greater extent on TikTok than on other platforms and potentially lead to body image disturbances, disordered eating behaviour, and other negative outcomes among young women.

General discussion

The findings of the current study are important but must also be understood within the broader context of participant’s daily lives beyond their participation in this study. Everyday female-identifying individuals are exposed to a multitude of different sources of information from which body image related stimuli can be drawn. The present study’s experiment was not conducted in a controlled environment due to its online nature, therefore researchers did not have the ability to assess and control for other pieces of body image-related information that participants might have consumed prior to participation that may have been salient for their body image. Further research is required to identify how sustained a change in body image states as measured by the BISS may be over time.

The findings of this study provide some insights into how social media influences disordered eating behaviour and mental health; a theoretical gap in the literature that Choukas-Bradley et al. [ 6 ] highlight as holding back research in this domain. In particular, the findings of the current study indicate that short periods of exposure to disordered TikTok content have an effect, while the high-range EAT-26 scores observed for those who engaged with TikTok for two or more hours a day also raise questions about the duration of exposure. Nonetheless, our findings demonstrate that short exposure periods are sufficient to have a negative effect on body image and internalisation of the thin ideal.

One point that may be readily overlooked in developing a theoretical framework around social media’s influence is that the narrative arc of TikTok videos is such that users are exposed to many short stories in quick succession, which may have a different effect to longer form content from a single content creator. As Pierce [ 2 ] notes, the speed of exposure to overlapping, but separate narratives depicted in successive videos, is an important feature of TikTok content and may contribute to the influence of such platforms on disordered eating and body attitudes. Each piece of content serves as a standalone narrative but may also overlap and interact with the viewer’s experience of the next video they watch to build a cumulative, normalised narrative of disordered body- and eating-practices.

In the current study, participants who engaged with TikTok for two-three hours a day were classified as high users, and those who used TikTok for three or more hours were classed as extreme. These rates of usage may, however, be quite normative, with Santarossa and Woodruff [ 73 ] citing three-four hours a day on social media as normative for their sample of young adults, though notably participants in the current study were only questioned about their TikTok usage, not their general use of social media.

While we examined the effect of pro-ana content in this study, that some changes were observed in the control group as well as the experimental group indicates that the social media environment, characterised as it is by idealisation, instant feedback, and readily available social comparison [ 6 ], may play a general role in diminishing positive body image attitudes and healthy aspirations. This is supported by Tiggemann and Slater’s [ 35 , 36 ] research in which social media usage was found to correlate positively with higher levels of body image concerns, in contrast to time spent on the internet more generally, and this may be particularly true for visually oriented platforms that sensitize viewers to their own appearance and that of others. As noted previously, of the visually-oriented social media platforms, predominantly TikTok and Instagram, videos are commonly framed on TikTok so that the subject’s whole body is visible, particularly in dance videos and in #GymTok content, where on Instagram, cameo style head-shot videos appear more likely to feature, which further suggests that TikTok may provide more body-related stimuli than other platforms, even when the intention of the content does not relate to body-image or #fitspiration.

Importantly, the algorithm on TikTok functions in such a way that those who actively seek out body positivity content may also be exposed to nefarious body-related content such as body checking, a competitive, self-surveillance type of content where users are encouraged to test out their weight by attempting to drink from a glass of water while their arm encircles another’s waist. As McGuigan [ 74 ] reports, watching just one body checking video may result in hundreds more filtering through a user’s For You page, with those actively attempting to seek out positive body image content likely to be inadvertently exposed to disordered content due to the configuration of the algorithm. This function of the For You page is demonstrated in the current study, with 64% of participants reporting having seen disordered eating content on their For You page, higher than any other kind of harmful content, including suicide and bullying. The current study did not assess participants’ consumption of #FoodTok, #GymTok, and #Fitspiration. Engagement with these dimensions of TikTok and the type of content that participants seek out via the search function warrant consideration in future research.

The TikTok algorithm underscores Logrieco et al’s. [ 18 ] findings that even anti-anorexia content can be problematic, especially given complexities in determining and controlling what is performatively problematic, including videos discussing recovery and positive body attitudes that may somewhat paradoxically further body policing and competition among users and consumers of social media content. Furthermore, as Logrieco et al. [ 18 ] highlight, TikTok is replete in both pro-ana and much more implicit body-related content that may be harmful to viewers, not to mention those creating the content, whose experiences also warrant consideration.

Theoretical and practical implications

The present study bridged an important gap in the literature by utilising both experimental and cross-sectional designs to examine the influence of pro-ana TikTok content on users’ body image satisfaction, internalisation of body ideals, and disordered eating behaviours. While the negative impact of social media on body image and eating behaviours has been established in relation to platforms such as Instagram and Twitter, TikTok’s rapid emergence and unique algorithm warrant independent analysis.

The present findings have important theoretical implications for the understanding of sociocultural influences of orthorexia nervosa development. Notably, this study is one of the first to highlight the association between orthorexia nervosa and the tripartite model of disordered eating using an experimental design. The results illustrate that the internalisation of sociocultural appearance ideals predicts the development of ‘healthy’ disordered eating, as suggested by the tripartite theory. Western culture ideals do seem to influence the expression of orthorexic tendencies, thus caution should be exercised by women when interacting with appearance-related TikTok content.

Unlike explicit pro-ana content, which is open to condemnation, the moral and health-related discourses underpinning much body-related content in which thinness and health are espoused as goodness, reflects a new trend in diet culture masquerading as wellness culture [ 20 , 21 ]. Questions are raised around the ethics of social media algorithms when the technologically fostered link between recovery-focused content and disordered-content on TikTok is laid bare, particularly considering that extant research has found individuals with experience of eating disorders often seek out support, safety, and connection online [ 49 ] and in doing so on a platform like TikTok, may be exposed to more disordered eating content than the average user. Given visual social media platforms are associated with higher levels of dysfunction in relation to body image [ 4 ], the policy and ethics of such platforms warrant scrutiny from a variety of stakeholders in management, marketing, technology regulation, with psychology playing an important role in the marketing of these platforms. As traditional journalistic platforms have been subjected to scrutiny and reform, so too must a climate of accountability be established within the social media nexus.

The widespread growth of social media may warrant greater concern than traditional forms of mass media, not only because of the full-time accessibility and diverse range of platforms, but also due to the prevalence of peer-to-peer interactions. According to the social comparison theory, comparison of oneself to others has traditionally considered more removed, higher status influences (e.g., celebrities, actors/actresses, supermodels) as a greater source of pressure than those in the individuals’ natural environment (e.g., family and peers). Re-examination of this theoretical perspective is warranted considering the contemporary challenges of social media and the perpetuation of body image messages from alike peers. Furthermore, a diverse range of “content” may trigger disordered body- and eating-related attitudes, including #fitspiration and #GymTok, which poses challenges for social media platforms in regulating content. The inclusion of orthorexia in the milieu highlights the disordered nature of seemingly benign health practices and social media content.

That TikTok content containing explicit and implicit pro-ana themes may readily remain on the app uncensored exemplifies the importance of protective strategies to build resilience at the individual level. One such protective strategy is shifting focus from body appearance to functionality. Alleva and colleagues [ 71 ] investigated the Expand Your Horizon programme, designed to improve body image by training women to focus on body functionality. They report that women who engaged with the Expand Your Horizon programme experienced greater satisfaction with body image and functionality, body appreciation, and reduced self-objectification compared to women who did not engage with the program. Health professionals involved in the care of women with eating disorders and other mental health issues should also be educated to ensure they are knowledgeable about the social media content their clients may be exposed to, equipping them with skills to engage in conversations about the potential detrimental impacts of viewing pro-ana and other harmful TikTok content [ 53 ].

The administration of such programs in schools, universities, community groups, and clinical settings could prove effectual in the prevention of disordered eating and body image disturbance development and may reduce symptom severity of a pre-established disorder. Such programs must be developed with great care, however, given the propensity for even anti-anorexia content to have a negative effect on those consuming it [ 18 ]. The development of self-compassion may also build resilience in women, with research confirming that self-compassion can be effectively taught [ 75 ]. Subsequently, programs have been developed such as Compassion Focused Therapy (CFT) in which clients are trained to develop more compassionate self-talk during negative thought processes and to foster more constructive thought patterns [ 76 ]. The value of CFT has been established in the literature with both clinical and non-clinical samples and has promising outcomes particularly for those high in self-criticism [ 77 ].

Young women should be provided with media literacy tools that can assist in advancing critical evaluations of the online world. Digital manipulation of advertising and celebrity images is well known to many people, however, this awareness may be lacking regarding social media images, as they are generally disseminated within one’s peer network rather than outside of it [ 33 ]. Media literacy interventions may educate women about how social media perpetuates appearance-ideals that are often unrealistic and unattainable [ 53 ]. As an example, Posavac et al. [ 78 ] revealed that a single media literacy intervention resulted in a reduction in women’s social comparison to body ideals portrayed in the media.

Such interventions might be extended to female-identifying TikTok users to educate them on the manipulation of videos to produce idealised portrayals of the self. Media literacy should be commenced from an early age by teaching children, adolescents, and adults to understand the influence of implicit messages conveyed through social media and to create media content that is responsible and psychologically safe for others [ 79 ]. Increased understanding of messages portrayed by social media content may prevent thin-ideal endorsement and internet misuse. Notably, however, the most effective approach would be to address the problem at its source and increase the regulation of social media companies, rather than upskill users in how to respond to harmful online environments, which creates further labour for the individual while allowing organisations to continue to produce harmful but easily monetizable content.

Limitations and future directions

To meet the requirements to run multivariate analyses, the continuous data of body image and internalisation scores were dichotomised using a median split to create ‘low’ and ‘high’ groups for each variable. Although dichotomisation was necessary to perform appropriate analyses and power analyses deemed the sample size as adequate following performance of the median split, dichotomising these variables may have contributed to a loss of statistical power to detect true effects.

Limitations are implicated in the use of the ORTO-15 in the present study. The ORTO-15 does not account for different lifestyle factors that may alter a participants’ response, such as dietary restrictions, food intolerances, or medical dietary guidelines. The discrepancies in literature surrounding the psychometric properties of the ORTO-15 may be attributable to the lack of established diagnostic criteria of orthorexia nervosa, cultural differences in expressions of eating disorders, and difficulty comparing research results in determining orthorexia nervosa diagnoses due to inconsistencies in testing questions and cut-off values [ 61 ]. Due to unacceptable reliability in the present study, a factor analysis was performed which identified a factor relating to health food preoccupation. This identified factor was used as the ORTO-15 measure and data from these 5-items were used in analyses and referred to throughout the present study as ‘healthy’ disordered eating. Using the 5-items related to ‘healthy’ disordered eating rather than the complete 15-item scale may not have accurately assessed participants’ degree of orthorexic tendencies. Despite these limitations, the ORTO-15 is the only accepted measure of orthorexic tendencies available [ 63 ]. Additionally, more limitations would likely have been encountered by using the full 15-item measure lacking reliability, compared to utilising the 5-item factor with acceptable reliability.

Future studies of TikTok and disordered eating behaviour should incorporate a measure of social comparison to verify whether social comparison is the vehicle through which women experience decreased body image satisfaction after viewing TikTok content. Future research should also examine the influence of TikTok content creation on body image, internalisation of thinness, and disordered eating behaviour and explore the association between what individuals consume on TikTok and the social media content that they produce. This research should be conducted using more diverse samples of women, including transgender women, to determine whether the findings of the present study are relevant for this population given the unique challenges regarding body image and societal beauty standards that they may experience.

Longitudinal studies are also warranted to examine the effect of exposure to pro-ana TikTok content over time, and to assess the effects of pro-ana TikTok content on body image satisfaction and eating disorder symptomology over time. Further research on orthorexia nervosa is needed to establish a more reliable measure of orthorexic tendencies and this would enable future investigation of the impact of pro-ana TikTok content on the development of orthorexia nervosa, as well as individual differences as predisposing factors in the development of orthorexic tendencies. Finally, future research should examine the efficacy of media literacy and self-compassion intervention programs as a protective factor specifically in the TikTok context, where disordered eating messages are more explicit in nature than traditional media and other social media platforms.

The findings of the current study support the notion that pro-ana TikTok content decreases body image satisfaction and increases internalisation of societal beauty standards in young women. This research is timely given reliance on social media for social interaction, particularly for young adults. Our findings indicate that female-identifying TikTok users may experience psychological harm even when explicit pro-ana content is not sought out and even when their TikTok use is time-limited in nature. The findings of this study suggest cultural and organisation change is needed. There is a need for more stringent controls and regulations from TikTok in relation to pro-ana content as well as more subtle forms of disordered eating- and body-related content. Prohibiting or restricting access to pro-ana content on TikTok may reduce the development of disordered eating and the longevity and severity of established eating disorder symptomatology among young women in the TikTok community. There are current steps being taken to delete dangerous content, including blocking searches such as “#anorexia”, however, there are various ways users circumvent these controls and further regulation is required. Unless effective controls are implemented within the platform to prevent the circulation of pro-ana content, female-identifying TikTok users may continue to experience immediate detrimental consequences for body image satisfaction, thin-ideal internalisation, and may experience an increased risk of developing disordered eating behaviours.

  • 1. Burger MR. Correlation Between Social Media Use and Eating Disorder Symptoms: A Literature Review. California Polytechnic State University. 2022. https://digitalcommons.calpoly.edu/kinesp/20/ .
  • 2. Pierce S. Alimentary Politics and Algorithms: The Spread of Information about “Healthy” Eating and Diet on TikTok. Washington University. 2022. https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/undergrad_etd/40\ .
  • 3. Asano E. How Much Time Do People Spend on Social Media? 2017 Jan 1 [cited 15 December 2022]. In: Social Media Today [Internet]. Industry Dive 2023. https://www.socialmediatoday.com/marketing/how-much-time-do-people-spend-social-media-infographic .
  • View Article
  • PubMed/NCBI
  • Google Scholar
  • 10. Green S. An Experimental Study on the Effects of Pro-Anorexia Content on Eating Disorder Development. M.A. Thesis, Western Kentucky University. 2019. https://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/3138 .
  • 13. Shepherd J. 20 Essential TikTok statistics you need to know in 2022. 2022 Oct 1 [cited 1 December 2022]. In: The Social Shephard [Internet]. 2023. https://thesocialshepherd.com/blog/tiktok-statistics .
  • 14. Kemp S. Digital 2020: Global Digital Overview. Datareportal. 2020 Jan 30 [Cited 2022 November 29]. https://datareportal.com/reports/digital-2020-global-digital-overview .
  • 15. TikTok. Privacy Policy. Updated 2023 Aug 4 [cited 5 May 2021]. https://www.tiktok.com/legal/page/row/privacy-policy/en .
  • 17. Muno DA. Orthorexia nervosa as a distinct eating disorder category: Similarities in alexithymia, attachment, perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, & eating attitudes. Psy.D. Dissertation, Alliant International University. 2020. https://www.proquest.com/openview/e8979ba30dd9eeefd80214e6556c1960/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y .
  • 23. Cash T. Cognitive-behavioural perspectives on body image. In: Cash T, Pruzinsky T, editors. Body Image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2002. pp. 38–46.
  • 37. DeMuynck JP. The Femininity Diet: A Rhetorical Analysis of the Discursive Formation of Femininity and Weight Loss in Contemporary Social Media Promotions. M.A. Thesis, Texas State University. 2020. https://digital.library.txst.edu/items/a15097cd-b076-4ccd-8f80-2b5d373550eb .
  • 48. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. American Psychological Association; 1999. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1999-02140-000 .
  • 64. Pallant J. SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS. 6 th ed. McGraw-hill education (UK); 2016.
  • 68. Cohen JW. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2 nd ed. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  • 70. Gibson KS. Thinspiration and Fatspiration on Body Dissatisfaction: The Roles of Social Comparisons and Anti-Fat Attitudes. M.A. Dissertation, Texas State University. 2021.
  • 72. Hülsing GM. # Triggerwarning: Body Image: A qualitative study on the influences of TikTok consumption on the Body Image of adolescents. BSc Thesis, University of Twente. 2021.
  • 74. McGuigan S. Body checking is dangerous and it’s all over TikTok. 2022 June 14 [cited 1 December 2022]. In Refinery29 [Internet]. 2022. https://www.refinery29.com/en-au/body-checking-tiktok-trend .
  • 76. Gilbert P. Compassion: Conceptualisations, research, and use in psychotherapy. London, UK: Routledge; 2005. https://books.google.com.au/books?id=-I6NAgAAQBAJ&dq=Gilbert,+P.+(2005).+Compassion:+Conceptualisations,+research,+and+use+in+psychotherapy&lr=&source=gbs_navlinks_s .

COMMENTS

  1. Experimental Psych Test 2 Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Every experimental hypothesis must be a(n) _____ statement., Since brightness can be directly measured using a light meter, brightness is a(n), Schachter (1959) measured affiliation using a _____ scale, since subjects selected among three categories: wait alone, wait with others, or no preference. and more.

  2. Chapter 6 Flashcards

    An experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ____ statement. Synthetic. Falsifiable means that a hypothesis is. capable of being disproven. ... Identify the synthetic statement. Women drive automobiles more safely than men. ____ is the basic tool that researchers use to build theories from empirical findings.

  3. Experimental Psych

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Every experimental hypothesis must be a(n) ______ statement., Which of the following statements is not testable?, The author has the hypothesis that her newest dog, Samson, is so dumb that he forgets who she is between the time she leaves for work and the time she gets home. One problem with this hypothesis is that it is probably ...

  4. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

    Examples. A research hypothesis, in its plural form "hypotheses," is a specific, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its outset. It is a key component of the scientific method. Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding.

  5. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    Developing a hypothesis (with example) Step 1. Ask a question. Writing a hypothesis begins with a research question that you want to answer. The question should be focused, specific, and researchable within the constraints of your project. Example: Research question.

  6. Hypothesis: Definition, Examples, and Types

    A hypothesis is a tentative statement about the relationship between two or more variables. It is a specific, testable prediction about what you expect to happen in a study. It is a preliminary answer to your question that helps guide the research process. Consider a study designed to examine the relationship between sleep deprivation and test ...

  7. Scientific hypothesis

    hypothesis. science. scientific hypothesis, an idea that proposes a tentative explanation about a phenomenon or a narrow set of phenomena observed in the natural world. The two primary features of a scientific hypothesis are falsifiability and testability, which are reflected in an "If…then" statement summarizing the idea and in the ...

  8. PDF DEVELOPING HYPOTHESIS AND RESEARCH QUESTIONS

    "A hypothesis is a conjectural statement of the relation between two or more variables". (Kerlinger, 1956) "Hypothesis is a formal statement that presents the expected relationship between an independent and dependent variable."(Creswell, 1994) "A research question is essentially a hypothesis asked in the form of a question."

  9. 2.4 Developing a Hypothesis

    Theories and Hypotheses. Before describing how to develop a hypothesis it is imporant to distinguish betwee a theory and a hypothesis. A theory is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena.Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions ...

  10. What is a Research Hypothesis: How to Write it, Types, and Examples

    It seeks to explore and understand a particular aspect of the research subject. In contrast, a research hypothesis is a specific statement or prediction that suggests an expected relationship between variables. It is formulated based on existing knowledge or theories and guides the research design and data analysis. 7.

  11. Experimental Hypothesis

    An experimental hypothesis is an educated guess or a prediction about the outcome of an experiment. ... In order to answer this question there must first be an understanding of what a hypothesis ...

  12. Guide to Experimental Design

    Table of contents. Step 1: Define your variables. Step 2: Write your hypothesis. Step 3: Design your experimental treatments. Step 4: Assign your subjects to treatment groups. Step 5: Measure your dependent variable. Other interesting articles. Frequently asked questions about experiments.

  13. The scientific method and experimental design

    A. The facts collected from an experiment are written in the form of a hypothesis. (Choice B) A hypothesis is the correct answer to a scientific question. B. A hypothesis is the correct answer to a scientific question. (Choice C) A hypothesis is a possible, testable explanation for a scientific question. C.

  14. Research Methods Quiz Chapter 1 Flashcards

    1)They attempt to organize a given body of scientific data.2)They point the way to new research. A good rule of thumb for distinguishing a statement of scientific theory from a scientific hypothesis is to look for general statements (in the case of theory) or for specific statements (in the case of hypothesis). true. "empirical".

  15. How to Write a Hypothesis in 6 Steps, With Examples

    4 Alternative hypothesis. An alternative hypothesis, abbreviated as H 1 or H A, is used in conjunction with a null hypothesis. It states the opposite of the null hypothesis, so that one and only one must be true. Examples: Plants grow better with bottled water than tap water. Professional psychics win the lottery more than other people. 5 ...

  16. Formulating Hypotheses for Different Study Designs

    Formulating Hypotheses for Different Study Designs. Generating a testable working hypothesis is the first step towards conducting original research. Such research may prove or disprove the proposed hypothesis. Case reports, case series, online surveys and other observational studies, clinical trials, and narrative reviews help to generate ...

  17. Experiments and Hypotheses

    Forming a Hypothesis. When conducting scientific experiments, researchers develop hypotheses to guide experimental design. A hypothesis is a suggested explanation that is both testable and falsifiable. You must be able to test your hypothesis, and it must be possible to prove your hypothesis true or false.

  18. Hypothesis Testing

    There are 5 main steps in hypothesis testing: State your research hypothesis as a null hypothesis and alternate hypothesis (H o) and (H a or H 1 ). Collect data in a way designed to test the hypothesis. Perform an appropriate statistical test. Decide whether to reject or fail to reject your null hypothesis. Present the findings in your results ...

  19. Research Hypothesis: Definition, Types, Examples and Quick Tips

    3. Simple hypothesis. A simple hypothesis is a statement made to reflect the relation between exactly two variables. One independent and one dependent. Consider the example, "Smoking is a prominent cause of lung cancer." The dependent variable, lung cancer, is dependent on the independent variable, smoking. 4.

  20. Chapter 6 Formulating the Hypothesis

    SYNTHETIC STATEMENTS are those that can be either true or false. each experimental hypothesis must be a synthetic statement so that there can be some chance it is true and some chance it is false. i."hungry students read slowly," it is a synthetic statement that can be supported or Contradicted.

  21. What Are the Elements of a Good Hypothesis?

    A hypothesis is an educated guess or prediction of what will happen. In science, a hypothesis proposes a relationship between factors called variables. A good hypothesis relates an independent variable and a dependent variable. The effect on the dependent variable depends on or is determined by what happens when you change the independent variable.

  22. What Is a Testable Hypothesis?

    Updated on January 12, 2019. A hypothesis is a tentative answer to a scientific question. A testable hypothesis is a hypothesis that can be proved or disproved as a result of testing, data collection, or experience. Only testable hypotheses can be used to conceive and perform an experiment using the scientific method .

  23. A hypothesis can't be right unless it can be proven wrong

    A hypothesis is considered scientific only if there is the possibility to disprove the hypothesis. The proof lies in being able to disprove. A hypothesis or model is called falsifiable if it is possible to conceive of an experimental observation that disproves the idea in question. That is, one of the possible outcomes of the designed ...

  24. #ForYou? the impact of pro-ana TikTok content on body image

    Videos glamourising disordered eating practices and body image concerns readily circulate on TikTok. Minimal empirical research has investigated the impact of TikTok content on body image and eating behaviour. The present study aimed to fill this gap in current research by examining the influence of pro-anorexia TikTok content on young women's body image and degree of internalisation of ...