ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Self-efficacy, satisfaction, and academic achievement: the mediator role of students' expectancy-value beliefs.

\r\nFernando Domnech-Betoret*

  • 1 Developmental and Educational Psychology, Jaume I University, Castellón, Spain
  • 2 Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Although there is considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, very few studies have explored the motivational mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy–achievement relationship, and they are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement. Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, this study examines the relationships among academic self-efficacy, students' expectancy-value beliefs, teaching process satisfaction, and academic achievement. Its main aim is to identify some motivational-underlying processes through which students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement and satisfaction. Student achievement and satisfaction are two of the most important learning outcomes, and are considered key indicators of education quality. The sample comprises 797 Spanish secondary education students from 36 educational settings and three schools. The scales that referred to self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were administered at the beginning of the course, while student satisfaction and achievement were measured at the end of the course. The data analysis was conducted by structural equation modeling (SEM). The results revealed that students' expectancy-value beliefs (Subject value, Process expectancy, Achievement expectancy, Cost expectancy) played a mediator role between academic self-efficacy and the achievement/satisfaction relationship. These results provided empirical evidence to better understand the mechanism that mediates self-efficacy–achievement and efficacy–course satisfaction relationships. The implications of these findings for teaching and learning in secondary education are discussed.

Introduction

Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, the main purpose of this study is to integrate self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs into predicting students' outcomes at secondary schools. This has barely been studied in previous research and sometimes with contradictory results.

Self-efficacy is a key personal variable of Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) Bandura's (1986) , defined as “an individual's belief in his or her own ability to organize and implement action to produce the desired achievements and results” ( Bandura, 1997 , p. 3). Educational researchers have paid plenty of attention to this construct (see Michaelides, 2008 , for a review). Prior studies have provided strong evidence that self-efficacy is a positive predictor of performance outcomes in different subjects ( Schunk et al., 2008 ; Usher and Pajares, 2008 ). For instance, Usher and Pajares ( 2008 , p. 751) argued that self-efficacy “predicts students' academic achievement across academic areas and levels.” Despite there being considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, studies that have explored the motivational mechanism which mediates self-efficacy–achievement relationship are scarce, and are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement, and will allow instructional actions and programs to improve academic achievement to be designed. One of the most solid proposals that integrate these variables is the social cognitive Expectancy-Value Model (E-VM) of achievement motivation, created by Eccles and her colleagues ( Eccles et al., 1983 ; Wigfield and Eccles, 1992 , Usher and Pajares (2000) based on Atkinson's (1964) expectancy-value model. This complex model includes multiple connections and components that can be classified into three main blocks/categories of variables, arranged in the following sequential order: social world, cognitive processes, and motivational beliefs. All these blocks of variables act directly or indirectly as predictors of students' achievement behavior, persistence, and choice. Centered on motivational beliefs, this model assumes that; first, expectancies for success (achievement expectancy is considered a component of expectancy for success) and subjective task values are directly related to achievement, task choices and persistence; and second, expectancies and task value are assumed to be influenced by individuals' goals and self-schemata. Self-efficacy or personal beliefs of competence is/are considered a salient aspect of self-schemata. Another model that shares similarities with E-VM is the Educational Situation Quality Model ( Doménech, 2006 , 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ; MOCSE is the acronym in Spanish) because: (a) both models are rooted in the social cognitive perspective of motivation; (b) they emphasize the important role that expectancy-value variables play in predicting students outcomes; (c) self-beliefs constructs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, self-confidence, etc.) are considered important antecedents of expectancy-value variables.

Based on the aforementioned theoretical frameworks, the purpose of this study is to test the validity of a structural model by integrating self-efficacy (adolescent students' self-belief) and expectancy and value constructs into predicting and explaining academic achievement and course satisfaction at secondary school. To examine how these motivational beliefs are related and affect such important students outcomes, they are important to not only design actions and programs to improve teacher effectiveness and students' academic results, but to also contribute to clarify the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value variables in predicting students outcomes, whose results available to date are limited and sometimes contradictory ( Williams, 2010 ).

The term “outcomes” may refer to cognitive and emotional variables. Regarding cognitive variables, learning achievements are considered the most important. As regards emotional variables, satisfaction with a course is an important outcome since it influences students' decisions to continue with or drop out of a course ( Levy, 2007 ). Satisfaction is also an important requirement for successful learning ( Sinclaire, 2014 ). The majority of the considered students' outcomes have to do with cognitive variables such as academic achievement (e.g., grades, test scores, etc.) or learning strategies. In the current study we have decided to include, besides academic achievement, an emotional dependent variable that has been less studied by authors in this tradition, such as, course satisfaction. Academic achievement and course satisfaction are considered two complementary learning outcome as the two face of the same coin. Teachers are interested in knowing not only if their student's progress, but also if they are satisfied with the T–L process followed. Both constructs are important indicators of the quality of the teaching-learning (T–L) process. Therefore, we believe that it would be interesting to test if the selected motivational variables differed in predicting and explaining both academic achievement and students' course satisfaction.

Regarding the relationship between self-efficacy and student satisfaction, Pajares and Schunk (2001) stated that a strong sense of efficacy enhances human well-being; for instance, self-efficacy beliefs influence the amount of stress and anxiety that people experience as they engage in an activity ( Pajares and Miller, 1994 ), and probably when students engage in a course. Self-efficacy also predicts course satisfaction in traditional face-to face classrooms ( Bandura, 1997 ). Although there is empirical evidence to support the positive effects of self-efficacy beliefs on students' well-being and course satisfaction ( DeWitz and Walsh, 2002 ), the motivational mechanisms that mediate the self-efficacy–students satisfaction relationship is still a problem to be solved. Very few studies have centered on examining the mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy–students' course satisfaction relationship, and are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' course satisfaction. These findings could provide important clues to promote student satisfaction. Student satisfaction is related to improved academic performance and the decision to take additional classes ( Booker and Rebman, 2005 ). Moreover, satisfaction at school is fundamental for the judgments that students make of their own general well-being ( Cummins and Tomyn, 2011 ).

Students' Expectancy-Value Beliefs

The Expectancy-value theory is grounded in the social cognitive perspective of motivation. Psychologists in this tradition argue that individuals' choice, persistence, and vigor expended in performance can be predicted and explained basically by expectations of achievement and the value attributed to a task; i.e., by their beliefs about how well they will do in the task and the extent to which they value the task ( Atkinson, 1957 ; Wigfield and Eccles, 1992 ; Wigfield, 1994 ). Apart from the components noted above, some theorists from this tradition have introduced a third construct related to the feelings experienced by students when they do a task ( Pintrich and De Groot, 1990 ). We name this third construct “process expectancy.” In the course/subject matter context, we understood process expectancy to be the positive feelings that students expect to experience in their interaction with their teacher during the course (How will I feel studying this subject?). Indeed experience tells us that no-one starts something that is not worthwhile or when expectations of success are very poor because completing the task in such circumstances is considered a waste of time. Finally, nobody starts a task if they do not expect be feel well during the performance process. Hence these beliefs are considered three important indicators of students' motivation ( Pintrich and De Groot, 1990 ), which specify some underlying motivational mechanisms that lead to the initiation and maintenance of action ( Pintrich and Schunk, 1996 ). Centered on a course-subject and based on the above arguments, we herein used three types of beliefs that can make adolescent students decide on striving to learn a subject or not: (a) the subject value (What value does this subject have for me?), (b) the achievement expectancy (Will I be able to pass this subject?), and (c) process expectancy (How will I feel studying this subject?).

The modern Expectancy-value theory ( Eccles and Wigfield, 2002 ; Eccles, 2009 ) distinguishes four task-value components: attainment value, intrinsic value, utility value and cost. For the present study, which centered on a course subject, we used extrinsic value (which encompasses utility, importance, and interestingness) and cost-benefit components to assess the subject matter value. Eccles and Wigfield (2002) identified cost as a critical component of value, which was conceptualized as a negative determinant in engaging a task due, for instance, to performance anxiety and fear of failure, and to the amount of effort needed to succeed ( Eccles and Wigfield, 2002 ). However, when we centered on a course, we understood that being involved in a specific subject depends not only on the time and effort invested, but also on the benefits (e.g., in terms of results, reinforcements, enjoyment, etc.) that students can obtain. In short, the subject value items employed in this work refer to: (a) the extrinsic subject value, i.e., the perceived utility, importance, and interestingness of the subject (What value does this subject have for me?); (b) the expected cost-benefit relationship to pass the subject (Will it be worth the time and effort that I will have to invest to pass the subject?).

Students' expectancy-value beliefs may have been generated before classes began, from previous experiences, or may arise on the first days of class when students meet the teacher and find out about the study syllabus, evaluation requirements, teacher methodology, etc. ( Doménech, 2006 , 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ). This means that these beliefs can be evaluated at the beginning of the course after some days/week of class.

The Mediator Role of Expectancy-Value Beliefs between Self-efficacy and the Achievement/Satisfaction Relationship

Regarding the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs.

When students face a new academic task, they ask themselves “Can I perform this task?” (self-efficacy) and “Why should I do this task?” (task value). If their answer to the first question is “yes,” they proceed to the next question ( Keskin, 2014 ). This reasoning suggests that self-efficacy is considered a predictor of task value, and not vice versa. Previous studies have demonstrated not only a positive relationship between both constructs ( Bong, 2001 ; Seo and Taherbhai, 2009 ), but also that self-efficacy is a direct predictor of task value ( Kozanitis et al., 2007 ; Azar et al., 2010 ; Keskin, 2014 ).

Prior research has also revealed significant and substantial direct effects of students' self-efficacy on academic expectations ( Chemers et al., 2001 ; Lent et al., 2008 ). According to these authors, students with high self-efficacy have greater academic expectations and display better academic performance that with low self-efficacy. These findings are consistent with what Bandura's postulated Bandura's (1997) when he argued that self-efficacy is causally prior to outcome expectancy as the results that individuals anticipate depend mainly on their judgments of how well they would be able to perform in a given situation ( Bandura, 1997 ). Therefore, it is assumed that self-efficacy (defined as the perceived capability to perform a given behavior) causally influences expected outcomes of behavior, but not vice versa.

In short, as regards the relationship between self-efficacy and the expectancy-value variables, the above-described findings support the notion that competence beliefs may drive students' expectations and task/subject values in the school context. However, more studies are needed to understand the connections between students' self-beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, etc.) and expectancy-value variables.

Regarding the Relationship between Expectancy-Value Beliefs and Achievement

Prior research has provided empirical evidence which indicates that expectancies and task-values are related to academic choices and achievement in specific domains, such as mathematics ( Marsh and Yeung, 1997 ; Spinath et al., 2004 ) and language arts ( Spinath et al., 2004 ). Recent cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have found that expectancy beliefs strongly influence achievement, whereas subject value considerably impacts choice, effort and persistence ( Nagengast et al., 2011 ; Gasco and Villarroel, 2014 ; Guo et al., 2015 ).

Regarding the Relationship between Expectancy-Value Beliefs and Satisfaction

Less is known about how students' expectancy-value beliefs relate to emotional outcomes, such as student satisfaction. Despite the findings being limited, they seem to support that satisfaction is well explained by task value ( Artino, 2008 ; Diep et al., 2016 ) and by grade expectancies ( Svanum and Aigner, 2011 ). Nonetheless, most authors highlight the teacher role and teacher–student interaction ( Wu et al., 2010 ) in relation to instructional and emotional supports as the main responsible factors of students' course satisfaction. Accordingly, process expectations, specifically related to the feelings that student experience during their interaction with the teacher, may play the most salient role to explain students' satisfaction. However, the process expectation formed by students can be influenced, in turn, by self-efficacy beliefs. Students with strong self-efficacy beliefs visualize success scenarios, which provide supportive resources, and guidance for performance ( Bandura, 1993 ). As a result, these students tend to experience more satisfaction with the teaching process than the students with low self-efficacy.

Finally, taken all de variables simultaneously, structural models tested in previous studies, based on the expectancy-value theory, provide additional and important evidence to support the mediator role played by motivational expectancy-value variables in the relationship between students' self-beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, etc.) and students outcomes. For example, the study conducted by Doménech-Betoret et al. (2014) in the university context revealed that students' academic self-efficacy had a significant and direct effect on achievement expectations, enjoyable learning expectations and expected dedication (cost) and, in turn, achievement expectation had a significant and direct effect on avoidance strategies (students' outcomes). In addition, subject value had a significant and direct effect on avoidance strategies (students' outcomes). In a similar vein, the study conducted by Bong et al. (2012) in the school context found that the task value and test anxiety significantly mediated the relationships of self-efficacy to achievement.

Based on the aforementioned empirical evidence, it is plausible to assume that the motivators beliefs which derive from the Expectancy-value theory may mediate the relationship between self-efficacy and learning outcomes; e.g., academic achievement and student satisfaction.

Objectives and Hypotheses

According to the aforementioned rationale, the main aim of this study was to examine if the motivational beliefs derived from the Expectancy-value theory play a mediator role between academic self-efficacy and learning outcomes (achievement and satisfaction). At the same time, another aim was to identify some of the motivational processes through which students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement and satisfaction (see Figure 1 ). Accordingly, we hypothesized that expectancy-value beliefs would have a direct effect on academic achievement and satisfaction, whereas academic self-efficacy would have an indirect effect on academic achievement and satisfaction through expectancy-value variables. In other words, we predicted that expectancy-value variables would play a mediator role between self-efficacy and achievement (H1), and between self-efficacy and satisfaction (H2). The hypothesized connections were addressed and tested by the Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) procedure with the EQS program ( Bentler, 2006 ). Self-efficacy and expectancy-value variables were all measured in the first academic term after some weeks of class, and student achievement and satisfaction were all measured in the third and last terms of the course. This study can provide new data to improve the motivational theories that integrate self-efficacy, expectancy, and value constructs in the educational setting context, focused on a subject matter as a unit of analysis. It may also help identify the motivational connections that mediate between academic self-efficacy and students' achievement/satisfaction. Important implications for educational practice may derive from these findings since they can provide valuable information to design instructional actions and programs that can improve student achievement and satisfaction. Student achievement and satisfaction are two of the most important learning outcomes, and are also considered key indicators of education quality.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 . Grafical representation of the study. *Underlying Motivational Mechanisms (UMM) are partially operationalized by the following questions: What value does this subject have for me?, How much time and effort will I invest to pass the subject?, Will I be successful in this subject?, and How will I feel studying this subject?.

Materials and Methods

Participants and procedure.

The sample consisted of 797 Spanish secondary education students from 36 classes with different subjects, of whom 404 were male (50.7%) and 393 were female (49.3%), and they were aged between 12 and 17 years. Most of their teachers ( N = 23, 63.88%) also participated in the study, of whom 11 were males (average experience = 29 years) and 12 were females (average experience = 32 years). About 80% of the participating students were Spanish, while the parents of the rest had come from other counties (the majority from Romania, Ecuador, and Morocco) to Spain as emigrants some years ago. One private and two state secondary schools located in east Spain took part in this study, which was carried out at the first four levels of compulsory secondary education: 1st ESO (12–13 years old), 2nd ESO (13–14 years old), 3rd ESO (14–15 years old), and 4th ESO (16–17 years old; ESO is the Spanish acronym for Educación Secundaria Obligatoria—Compulsory Secondary Education). Table 1 displays sample distribution according to levels of education and centers. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Regional Valencian Government, Spain. Consent for students to participate was required from students' parents or legal tutors. Confidentiality and personal data protection were guaranteed in accordance with current Spanish law.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 . Characteristics and sample distribution according to courses and centers.

The scales used to measure the variables considered herein have been reviewed and refined in previous studies ( Doménech, 2006 , 2012 , 2013 ; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ). As most had been designed for university students, they had to be adapted to secondary education in order to use them herein. The scales that referred to self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were administered at time 1 (halfway through the first term), and student satisfaction and achievement were measured at time 2 (halfway through the third term). See Table 2 for item examples.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 . Summary of the factor analysis, internal consistency and item example of the scales.

Students' General Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (25 Items, α = 0.86)

This scale is based on the original scales created by Bandura (1990) and by Pastorelli et al. (2001) . This scale was used to assess students' self-perception of how competent they were in the academic field. Students indicated their level of agreement within the 1 (very bad) to 4 (very good) range.

Expectancy-Value Scale (13 Items, α = 0.78)

This scale comprises 13 items and was designed to measure expectancy-value constructs at the beginning of the teaching-learning process. It was structured and designed according to the Motivational Theory proposed by Pintrich (1989) and Pintrich and De Groot (1990) . Students indicated their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale within the 1 (I am absolutely unconvinced) to 5 (I am absolutely convinced) range.

Satisfaction of the Teaching Process Scale (5 Items, α = 0.81)

The original scale was designed by Doménech (2011 , 2012) to assess university students' satisfaction with the teaching process followed in the classroom for a specific subject matter. The scale used herein was composed of five items and is a short version of the original teaching process scale adapted to secondary education. Students indicated their level of satisfaction with the teaching process, and opinions were viewed on a 4-point Likert scale within the 1 (unsatisfied) to 5 (very satisfy) range. Finally, student achievement was measured with the marks obtained by students for the first and second academic terms. The mark expected for the third term was also required. Achievement scores ranged from 1 (minimum) to 10 (maximum).

Statistical Analyses

The hypothesized connections were tested by the SEM procedure. The ML and ML robust method of estimation (if the assumption of multivariate normal distribution was violated), developed by Satorra and Bentler (1988 , 1994) , was used with the EQS program ( Bentler, 2006 ) to calculate the fit indices of the hypothesized models. Since the Chi-square test is sensitive to sample size, using relative fit indices like CFI, the NNFI, and RMSEA is highly recommended ( Bentler, 1990 ). Values below 0.05 for RMSEA indicate a good fit, whereas values up to 0.08 denote an unacceptable fit ( Browne and Cudeck, 1993 ). NNFI- and CFI-values above 0.90 ( Hoyle, 1995 ), or even 0.95 ( Hu and Bentler, 1999 ), were fixed as the cutting-off point.

Validity of the Measurement Model of Latent Variables

Hypothesized covariance structure models represent only approaches of reality because the obtained indices may be driven by the sample characteristics on which the model was tested ( Cudeck and Browne, 1983 ). One approach to mitigate this limitation is to employ the cross-validation strategy ( Byrne, 2012 ). To apply this strategy, the total sample ( N = 797) was randomly split into two equivalent subsamples (the calibration sample and the validation sample), following the recommendations of Cudeck and Browne (1983) . First, with subsample 1 ( n = 399), a separate explorative factor analysis (EFA), using the principal component method with varimax rotation, was conducted on all the scales to estimate their factorial structure. Second, by taking the factors extracted in the EFA as the observational variables, a separate confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed with subsample 2 ( n = 398) to test the goodness of fit and stability of the measurement models of these scales. These two-handed factorial analyses (exploratory and confirmatory) approach provide strong evidence for the reliability of the factors used as latent variables, and improve the validity of the measurement model ( Cudeck and Browne, 1983 ). Finally, the total sample ( N = 797) was then used to examine the structural model; i.e., the relationships among the latent variables (see Table 2 for details). When data analyses were performed, the initial sample slightly reduced because 23 students did not complete the entire scales. Missing values were not calculated given the large number of participants.

Students' General Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (25 Items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). Seven factors that corresponded to the seven academic skills included on the scale were extracted, which accounted for 62.17% of total variance. Cronbach's alpha values ranged between 0.82 (maximum) and 0.61 (minimum).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Sample 2). The fit indices values obtained using the ML (χ 2 = 573.459; p = 0.000, d.f . = 254; χ 2 / d.f . = 2.257; NFI = 0.831; NNFI = 0.878; CFI = 0.897; GFI = 0.892; AGFI = 0.861; RMSEA = 0.056) and ML Robust (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ 2 = 478.366; p = 0.000, d.f . = 254; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.883; NFI = 0.823; NNFI = 0.890; CFI = 0.907; IFI = 0.909; MFI = 0.754; RMSEA = 0.047) estimation methods indicated that the model fitted the data.

Expectancy-Value Scale (13 Items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). Four factors, which corresponded to the four scale constructs, were extracted, and accounted for 77.06% of total variance. Cronbach's alpha values ranged between 0.90 (maximum) and 0.79 (minimum).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Sample 2). The fit indices values obtained using the ML (χ 2 = 136.238; p = 0.000, d.f . = 94; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.449; NFI = 0.963; NNFI = 0.985; CFI = 0.988; GFI = 0.960; AGFI = 0.942; RMSEA = 0.034) and ML Robust (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ 2 = 117.663; p = 0.000, d.f . = 94; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.251; NFI = 0.963; NNFI = 0.990; CFI = 0.992; IFI = 0.992; MFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.025) estimation methods indicated that the model fitted the data.

Satisfaction with the Teaching Process Scale (5 Items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). One factor referred to satisfaction with the teaching process (α = 0.81), and was extracted and accounted for 56.05% of total variance. The confirmatory factorial analysis (Sample 2) was not applicable because only one factor was extracted.

Procedure for Testing Mediation

The structural equation analysis was carried out with the whole sample to firstly test the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between the self-efficacy–achievement relationship, and secondly the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between the self-efficacy–satisfaction relationship. The procedure followed to test the mediation effect of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and achievement, and also between self-efficacy and satisfaction, was conducted in two steps: first by testing the significant direct effects of latent variable self-efficacy on latent variables achievement (M1A model) and satisfaction (M1S model); second by testing the mediated role of the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs on the self-efficacy–achievement relationship (M2A), and also on the efficacy–satisfaction relationship (M2S). In this case we considered the direct and indirect effects between self-efficacy and achievement/satisfaction simultaneously.

Testing the Mediation Effect of Expectancy-Value Beliefs between self-Efficacy and Achievement (H1)

The M1A model was first tested (direct effects) for the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and achievement. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 194.52; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 34; NNFI = 0.93; CFI = 0.95; GFI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.078) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 173.19; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 34; NNFI = 0.93; CFI = 0.95; RMSEA = 0.073) indicated that the model satisfactorily fitted the data. According to the data, academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on achievement. So this prerequisite for mediation to exist was met ( Baron and Kenny, 1986 ).

Next the mediated model M2A was tested. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 329.77; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 74; NNFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.94; GFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.067) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 293.87; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 74; NNFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.062) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, latent variable academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on expectancy-value beliefs, which, in turn, had a significant effect on achievement. On the contrary, the path between academic self-efficacy and achievement was not significant. See Figure 2 for details.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 . M2A model (direct and indirect effects). Relationship among students' academic self-efficacy, expectancy-value beliefs, and achievement. The structural configuration and standardized coefficients of the model are displayed. *Significant ( p < 0.05), n.s., not significant.

Testing the Mediation Role of the Expectancy-Value Beliefs between the Self-Efficacy–Satisfaction Relationship (H2)

The M1S model was first tested (direct effects) for the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and satisfaction. The model was optimized when a covariance between two variable errors (E10–E12) from the self-efficacy latent variable was introduced, following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the model was tested again. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 197.88; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 52; NNFI = 0.926; CFI = 0.942; GFI = 0.959; RMSEA = 0.060) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 163.57; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 52; NNFI = 0.931; CFI = 0.946; RMSEA = 0.053) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, Academic Self-Efficacy had a significant effect on teaching process satisfaction. So this prerequisite for mediation to exist was met ( Baron and Kenny, 1986 ).

Second the mediated model M2S was tested. The model was optimized when a covariance between two variable errors (E10–E12) from the self-efficacy latent variable was introduced, following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the model was tested again. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 399.86; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 100; NNFI = 0.891; CFI = 0.909; GFI = 0.938; RMSEA = 0.062) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 343.17; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 100; NNFI = 0.894; CFI = 0.912; RMSEA = 0.056) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, the latent variable academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on expectancy-value beliefs which, in turn, had a significant effect on teaching satisfaction. On the contrary, the path between academic self-efficacy and teaching satisfaction was not significant. See Figure 3 for details.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 . M2S model (direct and indirect effects). Relationship among students' academic self-efficacy, expectancy-value beliefs, and teaching process satisfaction. The structural configuration and standardized coefficients of the optimized model are displayed. *Significant ( p < 0.05), n.s., not significant.

The description and fit indices of the tested models are provided in Table 3 , which summarizes the structural equation analyses results.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 . Fit indices of the tested models ( N = 797).

Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, this study examines; first, the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement; second, the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction with the teaching process. Achievement and satisfaction were considered dependent variables in two separate models as indicators of teaching practice quality.

For the mediator role played by the expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement, and following the recommendation by Baron and Kenny (1986) for testing mediation, the prediction capacity of students' academic self-efficacy on student achievement was examined first by testing the M1A model; second the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement was examined by testing the M2A model. According to the M1A model, the structural analyses indicated a direct, positive and significant effect of the latent variable academic self-efficacy on achievement. According to the M2A model, the obtained fit indices supported the hypothesized connections. This means that the expectancy-value beliefs mediated the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement. These results indicated that students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement, but only indirectly; i.e., by fulfilling the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs.

For the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction with the teaching process, and following the recommendation by Baron and Kenny (1986) for testing mediation, the prediction capacity of students' academic self-efficacy on student satisfaction was examined first by testing the M1S model; second the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction was examined by testing the M2S model.

In accordance with the M1S model, the structural analyses indicated a positive and significant direct effect of students' academic self-efficacy on the latent variable teaching process. According to the M2S model, the obtained fit indices supported the hypothesized connections. This means that the expectancy-value beliefs mediated the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction. These results suggested that students' academic self-efficacy affects student satisfaction, but only indirectly; i.e., by fulfilling the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs.

Given the remarkable variance explained in both models (M2A and M2S), it can be stated that general academic self-efficacy has a strong effect on expectancy-value beliefs. These findings indicated that the level of activation and quality of students' expectancy-value beliefs during the first weeks of the teaching-learning process (after some weeks attending class) depended to a great extent on the evaluation that students made of their own academic skills/capabilities (self-beliefs); e.g., study techniques, planning study, team work skills, coping with new technologies, memorization capacity, oral and written communication, and coping with exam situations. In the light of the obtained results, academic self-efficacy can be considered an important internal source of motivation that is capable of activating students' motivation in the first stage of the behavioral process; i.e., academic self-efficacy contributes to a great extent to activate student students' motivation from the first weeks of the teaching-learning process undertaken with a specific subject. Therefore, it is important to take into account students' academic self-efficacy when they face a new educational setting. These findings are similar to others obtained in previous research. Thus, the study conducted by Doménech-Betoret et al. (2014) revealed the key role played by academic self-efficacy in explaining students' expectations (achievement expectations, enjoyable learning expectations, and expected dedication according to the subject value). In a similar vein, the structural model tested by Bong et al. (2012) revealed that self-beliefs (self-efficacy and self-concept) are good predictors of task value.

Expectancy-value beliefs had a direct positive and significant effect on student achievement/satisfaction. These findings suggested that expectancy-value beliefs (Achievement expectations, Value of the subject matter, Process expectations with the teacher, Expected cost to pass the subject), which were evaluated some weeks after the course began, would be capable of satisfactorily explaining and predicting student achievement and their degree of satisfaction with the teaching process followed with a specific subject matter. The observational variables with higher loadings (Achievement expectations, Value of the subject and Satisfaction expectations with the process) on the latent factor expectancy-value beliefs suggested that these motivational variables were the most important predictors of student achievement and satisfaction. These findings fall in line with previous studies that used the variables from the Expectation-value theory ( Guo et al., 2015 ). The structural model tested by these authors evidenced that Math self-concept (construct used to assess students' expectancy of success) and the Math utility value had a significant and direct effect on students' academic achievement and educational aspirations.

All these findings moved in the expected direction. Academic self-efficacy, considered a general domain variable ( Boekaerts, 1999 ), predicted and explained students' specific expectancy-value beliefs in connection with a specific educational setting. In turn, these specific expectancy-value beliefs predicted/explained students' outcomes (academic achievement and satisfaction). These results were coherent with what Bandura postulated when claiming that specific measures of beliefs were more closely related to behavior ( Bandura, 1997 ).

Based on the obtained results, the following conclusions can be drawn:

(a) Expectancy-value beliefs, understood as the anticipatory previsions and forecasts that students make in an attempt to anticipate their actions, emotions and results in a new educational situation, were well measured and operationalized by the four motivational selected factors that derived from the Expectancy-value theory: Value of the subject, achievement expectancy, satisfaction expectancy with the process, and the expected cost to pass the subject.

(b) Students' expectancy-value beliefs, generated/activated during the first weeks of the teaching-learning process, were well explained by the perception or idea that students form about their own basic academic skills. These findings also fall in line with previous studies ( Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ).

(c) Four motivational variables, which mediate the relationship between academic self-efficacy and students' achievement/satisfaction, were identified. These findings fall in line with previous studies, which examined the mediator role of motivational variables in the relationship between self-efficacy and achievement ( Bong et al., 2012 ).

(d) These findings shed light to better understand the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs in predicting students' outcomes in secondary education.

(e) This study allows advances to be made in explaining students' emotional outcomes, such as course satisfaction, which has barely been studied in previous research in the expectancy-value theory context.

(f) Important educational implications can be derived from the socio-cognitive perspective of motivation acquired from the results obtained to improve students' achievement and satisfaction from a preventive point of view.

Educational Implications

Based on the results obtained in this study, the following educational implications can be made:

First, the expectancy-value beliefs generated during the first weeks of the course are capable of predicting student achievement and their satisfaction with the teaching process followed throughout the course. Therefore, we wish to stress the importance of making a diagnostic evaluation at the beginning of the course of secondary students' expectancy-value beliefs in order to: (a) detect possible shortcomings that students may present in relation to students' expectancy-value beliefs formed at the beginning of the course, after some days of class; (b) design an action plan to overcome or improve these shortcomings.

Second, the obtained results provide evidence that general academic self-efficacy is capable of explaining to a great extent the expectancy-value beliefs formed by secondary students about a specific subject some days after the course starts. Therefore teachers should also take it into account at the very beginning of the course. Accordingly, implementing actions and programs at schools is recommended to improve students' academic skills to, in turn, improve academic self-efficacy. These programs should include a variety of components that fall in line with the sources of self-efficacy beliefs proposed by Bandura (1997) in academic contexts. According to Bandura (1997) , self-efficacy beliefs are developed when individuals interpret information from four major sources, such as mastery experience, vicarious experience of observing others, social persuasions that students receive from others, and emotional and psychological states. In the school context, mastery experience refers to the way students interpret and evaluate obtained results, and self-beliefs of competence are revised and created according to these interpretations. Accordingly, teachers should provide instructional scenarios in which students are able to succeed in challenging tasks. Students' judgments of competence are also created by vicarious experience; i.e., by evaluating their capabilities in relation to other students' performance. Another source of self-efficacy is the social persuasions that students receive from others. Accordingly, a supportive message from parents and teachers is important to empower students' self-confidence. Finally, students' self-efficacy is created by their emotional and psychological states as students tend to interpret negative psychological states (stress, anxiety, bad mood, depression, etc.) as evidence for lack of skills, and positive psychological states as indicators of personal competence. Accordingly, promoting students well-being and reducing negative emotional states strengthen students' self-efficacy. For more details about sources of self-efficacy, see the review by Usher and Pajares (2008) . In short, the actions and programs that aim to develop students' self-efficacy should be based on these four sources.

We defend the notion of quality education based on a preventive view. Accordingly, we suggest secondary school teachers taking specific actions on the first days of the T–L process to improve adolescent students' beliefs as regards academic self-efficacy (self-beliefs), achievement expectancy, process expectancy and subject value.

(a) It is important for teachers to strive to transmit the idea that all the students in class are capable of passing the subject matter. This relates with students' psychological need of competence ( Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).

(b) From the very beginning of the course, improve students' perception of their own capacity, specifically the general academic skills required to improve progress made at school; e.g., taking actions to bridge some basic gaps in training that some students may still have from former courses, and are necessary to make progress in the subject; or evaluate and recognize the progress made by students since the evaluative feedback that students receive contribute to develop their competence. This point relates with students' psychological need of competence ( Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).

(c) Explain to students the value of the subject matter when presenting the subject matter syllabus, and also throughout the course. Inform students about the importance and usefulness of the subject matter (present or future) at the personal, academic and professional levels.

(d) From the very beginning of the course, promote and take care of the teacher–students interpersonal relationship; e.g., show closeness, respect, and empathy with students throughout the course. This relates with students' psychological need of relatedness ( Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).

Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research

Although the results obtained herein are satisfactory, some limitations and suggestions for future research should be pointed out.

First, the results were obtained from schools located in a specific socio-cultural context. Thus, replicating this study in other educational and cultural contexts is recommended to generalize the findings.

Second, extending the tested model should be considered first by introducing other types of self-efficacy, such as metacognitive self-efficacy ( Moores et al., 2006 ) or emotion regulation self-efficacy ( Gross and John, 2003 ); second by including other motivational variables as mediators. Regarding the motivational process, Bandura (1986) distinguished three types of cognitive motivators: (a) causal attributions; (b) outcomes expectations; (c) goals, whose corresponding theories are Attribution theory, Expectancy-value theory, and Goal theory. Accordingly, including new variables as mediators in future research, such as, goal orientation ( Pintrich, 2000 ) or achievement goals, would be interesting ( Liem et al., 2008 ).

Third, although academic self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were measured in the same data collection wave, we provide enough evidence and theoretical support to consider general academic self-efficacy as an antecedent of the students' expectancy-value belief generated in the classroom context.

Fourth, according to Wigfield and Cambria (2010) , most of the measures used by researchers to assess motivational beliefs are student self-report measures. However, self-report measures can be problematic, especially for young children or for students who state that school is not important to them. Consequently, we wish to emphasize the importance of combining quantitative and qualitative methods to reduce biases and to obtain more complete information about students' belief.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Regional Government of Valencia, Spain, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

All authors made substantial contributions to design the work, analysis and interpretation of the data, drafting and revising the work, final approval of the version to be published, and finally agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in insuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any aspects of the work were appropriately investigated and resolved (FD, LA, and AG).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Artino, A. R. (2008). Motivational beliefs and perceptions of instructional quality: predicting satisfaction with online training. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 24, 260–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00258.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol. Rev . 64(6 Pt 1), 359–372. doi: 10.1037/h0043445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An Introduction to Motivation . Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

Google Scholar

Azar, H. K., Lavasani, M. G., Malahmadi, E., and Amani, J. (2010). The role of self- efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning approaches and mathematics achievement. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 5, 942–947. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.214

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and action: A Social Cognitive Theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1990). “Mechanisms of moral disengagement,” in Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies, Theologies, States of mind , ed W. Reich (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 161–191.

Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educ. Psychol. 28, 117–148. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control . New York, NY: Freeman.

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychology research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Fit indexes, langrage multipliers, constraint changes and incomplete data in structural models. Multivariate Behav. Res. 25, 163–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_3

Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual . Ecino, CA: Multivarite Sotware Inc.

Boekaerts, M. (1999). Motivated learning: studying student situation transactional units. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 14, 41–55. doi: 10.1007/BF03173110

Bong, M. (2001). Between- and within-domain relations of academic motivation among middle and high school students: self-efficacy, task-value, and achievement goals. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 23–34. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.23

Bong, M., Cho, C., Ahn, H. S., and Kim, H. J. (2012). Comparison of self-beliefs for predicting student motivation and achievement. J. Educ. Res. 105, 336–352. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2011.627401

Booker, Q. E., and Rebman, C. E. (2005). E-Student retention: factors affecting customer loyalty for online program success. Issues Inform. Syst. 6, 183–189.

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models , eds K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage), 136–162.

Byrne, B. (2012). Structural Equation Modeling with Mplus. Basic Concepts, Apllications and Programing . New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.

Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., and Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year college student performance and adjustment. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 55–64. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.55

Cudeck, R., and Browne, M. W. (1983). Cross-validation of covariance structures. Multivariate Behav. Res. 18, 147–167. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1802_2

Cummins, R., and Tomyn, A. (2011). The subjective well being of high-school students: validating the personal wellbeing index-school children. Soc. Indic. Res. 101, 405–418. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9668-6

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuit: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum.

DeWitz, S. J., and Walsh, W. B. (2002). Self-efficacy and college student satisfaction. J. Career Assess. 10, 315–326. doi: 10.1177/10672702010003003

Diep, A. N., Zhu, Ch., Struyven, K., and Blieck, Y. (2016). Who and What contributes to student satisfaction in different blended learning modalities? Br. J. Educ. Technol. 48, 473–489. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12431

Doménech, F. (2006). Testing an instructional model in a university educational setting from the student's perspective. Learn. Instr. 16, 450–466. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.005

Doménech, F. (2011). Evaluar e Investigar en la Situación Educativa Universitaria. Un Nuevo Enfoque Desde el Espacio Europeo de Educación Superior. [Evaluate and Investigate in the University Educational Setting. A New Approach from the Higher European Area]. Publicacions de la Universitat Jaume I, Universitas. 34.

Doménech, F. (2012). Psicología Educativa: Su Aplicación al Contexto de la Clase [Educational Psychology: its Application in the Classroom Context] , Vol. 13. Castellón: Publicaciones de la Universitat Jaume I. Col·lecció Psique.

Doménech, F. (2013). An instructional model for guiding reflection and research in the classroom: the educational situation quality model. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 11, 239–260.

Doménech-Betoret, F., Gómez-Artiga, A., and Lloret-Segura, S. (2014). Personal variables, motivation and avoidance learning strategies in undergraduate students. Learn. Individ. Differ. 35, 122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.06.007

Eccles, J. (2009). Who am i and what am i going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action. Educ. Psychol. 44, 78–89. doi: 10.1080/00461520902832368

Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., et al. (1983). “Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors,” in Achievement and Achievement Motivation , ed J. T. Spence (San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman) 75–146.

Eccles, J. S., and Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 109–132. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153

Gasco, J., and Villarroel, J. D. (2014). The motivation of secondary school students in mathematical word problem solving. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 12, 83–106. doi: 10.14204/ejrep.32.13076

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

Guo, J., Marsh, H. W., Parker, P. D., Morin, A. J. S., and Yeung, A. S. (2015). Achievement, motivation, and educational choices: a longitudinal study of expectancy and value using a multiplicative perspective. Dev. Psychol. 51, 1163–1176. doi: 10.1037/a0039440

Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural Equation Modeling . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: coventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

Keskin, H. K. (2014). A path analysis of metacognitive strategies in reading, self-efficacy and task value. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. 4, 798–808.

Kozanitis, A., Desbiens, J.-F., and Chouinard, R. (2007). Perception of teacher support and reaction towards questioning: its relation to instrumental help-seeking and motivation to learn. Int. J. Teach. Learn. High. Educ. 19, 238–250.

Lent, R. W., Sheu, H. B., Singley, D., Schmidt, J. A., Schmidt, L. C., and Gloster, C. S. (2008). Longitudinal relations of self-efficacy to outcome expectations, interests, and major choice goals in engineering students. J. Vocat. Behav. 73, 328–335. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2008.07.005

Levy, Y. (2007). Comparing dropouts and persistence in e-learning courses. Comput. Educ. 48, 185–204. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2004.12.004

Liem, A. D., Lau, S., and Nie, Y. (2008). The role of self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning strategies, task disengagement, peer relationship, and achievement outcome. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33, 486–512. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.08.001

Marsh, H. W., and Yeung, A. S. (1997). Causal effects of academic self-concept on academic achievement: structural equation models of longitudinal data. J. Educ. Psychol. 89, 41–54. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.41

Michaelides, M. P. (2008). Emerging themes from early research on self-efficacy beliefs in school mathematics. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 6, 219–234.

Moores, T. T., Chang, J. C., and Smith, D. K. (2006). Clarifying the role of self-efficacy and metacognition as indicators of learning: construct development and test. Database Adv. Inform. Syst. 37, 125–132. doi: 10.1145/1161345.1161360

Nagengast, B., Marsh, H. W., Scalas, L. F., Xu, M. K., Hau, K. T., and Trautwein, U. (2011). Who took the “x” out of expectancy-value theory? A psychological mystery, a substantive-methodological synergy, and a cross-national generalization. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1058–1066. doi: 10.1177/0956797611415540

Pajares, F., and Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical problem solving: a path analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 86:193. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.193

Pajares, F., and Schunk, D. H. (2001). “Self-beliefs and school success: self-efficacy, selfconcept, and school achievement,” in Perception , eds R. Riding and S. Rayner (London: Ablex Publishing), 239–266.

Pastorelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Rola, J., Rozsa, S., and Bandura, A. (2001). The structure of children's perceived self-efficacy: a cross-national study. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 17, 87–97. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.17.2.87

Pintrich, P. L., and Schunk, D. H. (1996). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, and Applications . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Pintrich, P. R. (1989). “The dynamic interplay of student motivation and cognition in the college classroom,” in Advances in Motivation and Achievement , Vol. 6, eds C. En Ames and M. L. Maher (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press), 117–160.

Pintrich, P. R. (2000). “The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning,” in Handbook of Self-regulation , eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 452–502.

Pintrich, P. R., and De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 33–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (1988). “Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. ASA 1988,” in Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics, Section , (Alexandria, VA: American Statistical Association), 308–313.

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (1994). “Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis,” in Latent variables analysis: Applications for Developmental Research , eds A. von Eye and C. C. Clogg (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 399–419.

Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., and Meece, J. L. (2008). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research and Applications, 3rd Edn. , Upper saddle River, NJ: Merrill-Prentice Hall.

Seo, D. C., and Taherbhai, H. (2009). Motivational beliefs and cognitive processes in mathematics achievement, analyzed in the context of cultural differences: a Korean elementary school example. Asia Pac. Educ. Rev. 10, 193–203. doi: 10.1007/s12564-009-9017-0

Sinclaire, J. K. (2014). An empirical investigation of student satisfaction with college courses. Res. High. Educ. J. 22, 1–21.

Spinath, B., Spinath, F. M., Harlaar, N., and Plomin, R. (2004). Predicting school achievement from general cognitive ability, self-perceived ability, and intrinsic value. Intelligence 34, 363–374. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.004

Svanum, S., and Aigner, C. (2011). The influences of course effort, mastery and performance goals, grade expectancies, and earned course grades on student ratings of course satisfaction. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 81, 667–679. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02011.x

Usher, E. L., and Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: critical review of the literature and future directions. Rev. Educ. Res. 78, 751–796. doi: 10.3102/0034654308321456

Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: a developmental perspective. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 6, 49–78. doi: 10.1007/BF02209024

Wigfield, A., and Cambria, J. (2010). Students' achievement values, goal orientations, and interest: definitions, development, and relations to achievement outcomes. Dev. Rev. 30, 1–35. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.001

Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (1992). The development of achievement task values: a theoretical analysis. Dev. Rev. 12, 265–310. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90011-P

Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015

Williams, D. M. (2010). Outcome expectancy and self-efficacy: theoretical implications of an unresolved contradiction. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14, 417–425. doi: 10.1177/1088868310368802

Wu, H., Tennyson, R. D., and Hsia, T. (2010). A study of of student satisfaction in a blended e-learning system environment. Comput. Educ. 55, 155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2009.12.012

Keywords: self-efficacy, expectancy-value theory, expectancy beliefs, value beliefs, academic achievement, student satisfaction

Citation: Doménech-Betoret F, Abellán-Roselló L and Gómez-Artiga A (2017) Self-Efficacy, Satisfaction, and Academic Achievement: The Mediator Role of Students' Expectancy-Value Beliefs. Front. Psychol . 8:1193. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01193

Received: 01 May 2017; Accepted: 30 June 2017; Published: 18 July 2017.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2017 Doménech-Betoret, Abellán-Roselló and Gómez-Artiga. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fernando Doménech-Betoret, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

  • DOI: 10.1207/s15326985ep2603&4_2
  • Corpus ID: 5285325

Self-Efficacy and Academic Motivation

  • Published 2002
  • Psychology, Education

461 Citations

Relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation, the role of self-efficacy in improving student academic motivation, academic self-concept and self-efficacy: how different are they really.

  • Highly Influenced

Academic self-efficacy: from educational theory to instructional practice

The relationship between academic motivation and self-efficacy in undergraduate students: kosovo case, role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college students' course performance and future enrollment intentions., a reevaluation of the role of anxiety: self‐efficacy, anxiety, and their relation to reading and listening proficiency, trajectories of self-efficacy and achievement goals: a developmental approach, the effect of self-regulated learning strategies and self-efficacy on academic achievement of primary school students, the relationship between self-concept, self-efficacy, and military skills and abilities, 72 references, self-efficacy and achievement behaviors.

  • Highly Influential
  • 16 Excerpts

Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance.

A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences., self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change., enhancing self-efficacy and achievement through rewards and goals: motivational and informational effects, cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation., effect of self-efficacy, goals, and task strategies on task performance., the mediating role of attribution and self-efficacy variables for treatment effects on achievement outcomes., peer models : influence on children's self-efficacy and achievement, motivational processes affecting learning., related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Self-Efficacy Theory

Profile image of Medel Cabalsa

Self-efficacy has apparently served as an effective predictor of student’s learning and motivation for the last two decades. There has been a growing interest for research about self-efficacy in many setting. It is psychometrically from related motivational constructs. It is believe to be found a sensitive to elusive changes in student’s performance context, to interact with self regulated learning processes and act as intervening factors on student’s academic achievement. Bandura’s self-efficacy theory offers a social view of the extent of beliefs on personal ability to complete a task in order to reach the goals. This self-efficacy theory impacts every part of human endeavor. This theory as applied to academic context affects human function not just on behavior but academic achievement as well. A critique of this theory indicates that this theory can be useful as a basic motive to learn for students and need to consider the factors affecting to be successful. It can also be concluded that many more studies need to be done applying this model as for students learning motivation. Keywords: Self-efficacy, motivational constructs

Related Papers

Journal ijmr.net.in(UGC Approved)

Self-efficacy and motivation to learn are the important factors that affect the academic performance of an individual/student. Over the last years researchers have found nine commonly researched psychosocial constructs among which academic self-efficacy was and has been the strongest single predictor of student’s academic achievement and performance. Self-efficacy belief has been used as a performance- based measure thereby as an influence to motivate the academic performance of the student. This article explains the influence and interrelationship of self-efficacy as a motivator and as a contributor to the academic performance of the individual. INTRODUCTION Self-efficacy is based on Social Learning Theory a construct that affects motivation that promotes or inhibits learning. The Social Learning Theory (1960s) by Albert Bandura developed into the Social Cognitive Theory in 1986, which is based on self-efficacy belief. Self-efficacy belief provides the foundation for human – motivation, well-being and personal accomplishment. The theory explained, learning occurs in a social context with a dynamic and reciprocal interaction of the person, environment, and behaviour. It emphasizes on how these factors interact to determine motivation and behaviour. It considers the unique way in which an individual performs a behaviour also considering the social environment in which the individual performs the behaviour Bandura (1997).The first five constructs of Social learning theory was reciprocal determinism

self efficacy research paper pdf

OJMR Journal

Maria DiBenedetto

Journal of Educational, Health and Community Psychology

Triantoro Safaria

Earlier researches show effects of self-efficacy on students’ learning and achievement. Self- efficacy has operationally defined as one’s belief that people can successfully perform a given task. The main purpose of the paper is to discuss how self-efficacy developed and the way it influences students’ academic performance in addition to social interaction with peers. A scenario was given to Pakistani schools’ student by solving mathematical problems. Present study was designed to study the impact of self-efficacy on 15 boys, students of the 5th grade of a local school. Hague’s (1990) Urdu Self-efficacy scale was administered. It was found that students with high self-efficacy obtained higher scores on 50 mathematical problems test. Further, content analysis of interviewees’ responses showed that students with high self-efficacy planned to study complex subjects in future. A cross-cultural study is strongly recommended in this issue that determines the students’ future. Keyword: Sel...

Educational psychologist

Genaro Escate

Journal Universitas Muhammadiyah Gresik Engineering, Social Science, and Health International Conference (UMGESHIC)

Nadhirotul laily

This research is motivated by the emergence of the phenomenon of students experiencing a decline in grades. Academic achievement is a skill and ability as an achievement which is considered as a measure of student success. One of the factors that influence academic achievement is self-efficacy. To determine the relationship between self-efficacy and student achievement. Using quantitative research methods with a population of 152 students. Researchers used non-probability sampling technique with incidental sampling technique, obtained 58 samples. Data collection in this study used a scale (questionnaire/questionnaire) via google form using a Likert scale and documentation method in the form of IPS scores (Semester Achievement Index) to measure student achievement. The data analysis technique used Spearman Rho correlation test with SPSS 25.0 program. The results of this study indicate that there is no relationship between self-efficacy and student achievement. This research can be us...

Rani Asmara

Academic self-efficacy is receiving increasing recognition as a predictor of educational performances. Academic self-efficacy describes "a person's confidence in their abilities to organize, execute and regulate performance to attain designated types of performances". It functions at a multilevel and multifaceted set of beliefs that influence how people feel, think, motivate themselves, and behave during various educational tasks. Self-Efficacy view point can be built up on four bases of knowledge, vicarious experiences, enactive mastery as well as physical and emotional states. Self-efficacy development is closely intertwined with a person's experiences, competencies and developmental tasks in different domains at different stages in life. Self-efficacy beliefs should be relevant for understanding educational outcomes because self-efficacy leads to specific behaviours and motivations that can encourage or discourage effective performance.

International journal of academic research in business & social sciences

SITI NURANI ZULKFLI

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

rahma syarif

Syed Hasan Qasim

Wayamba Journal of Management

Ms. S . Priyadharsan

Lviv University Herald. Series: Psychological sciences

Olena Musakovska

Maria Dibenedetto , Dale Schunk Dhschunk

The Open Psychology Journal

Muwaga Musa

KnE Social Sciences

Ika wahyu pratiwi

bushra akram

Konstantinos Nassos

Search Results Web results Journal of Global Research in Education and Social Science

Jhoselle Tus

Gaurav Agrawal

Zenodo (CERN European Organization for Nuclear Research)

Mousa Matovu

noris Nurul ainiyah

IOSR Journal of Humanities and Social Science

ALI GARBA KOLO

Ahmed Elhassan H A M I D H A S S A N Hassan , Abdulaziz Alasmari

Journal of Educational and Social Research

kingsley chinaza nwosu

Ahmed Elhassan H A M I D H A S S A N Hassan , ELDOOD AHMED

A. Seda Saracaloğlu

Nasir Iqbal

accounts ziraf

JUDE AKOMOLAFE

Educational Psychology Review

Einar Skaalvik

Journal of Applied Psychology

Jeffrey Vancouver

Language Learning

Nicole Mills

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Self-Efficacy, Satisfaction, and Academic Achievement: The Mediator Role of Students' Expectancy-Value Beliefs

Fernando doménech-betoret.

1 Developmental and Educational Psychology, Jaume I University, Castellón, Spain

Laura Abellán-Roselló

Amparo gómez-artiga.

2 Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Although there is considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, very few studies have explored the motivational mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy–achievement relationship, and they are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement. Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, this study examines the relationships among academic self-efficacy, students' expectancy-value beliefs, teaching process satisfaction, and academic achievement. Its main aim is to identify some motivational-underlying processes through which students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement and satisfaction. Student achievement and satisfaction are two of the most important learning outcomes, and are considered key indicators of education quality. The sample comprises 797 Spanish secondary education students from 36 educational settings and three schools. The scales that referred to self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were administered at the beginning of the course, while student satisfaction and achievement were measured at the end of the course. The data analysis was conducted by structural equation modeling (SEM). The results revealed that students' expectancy-value beliefs (Subject value, Process expectancy, Achievement expectancy, Cost expectancy) played a mediator role between academic self-efficacy and the achievement/satisfaction relationship. These results provided empirical evidence to better understand the mechanism that mediates self-efficacy–achievement and efficacy–course satisfaction relationships. The implications of these findings for teaching and learning in secondary education are discussed.

Introduction

Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, the main purpose of this study is to integrate self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs into predicting students' outcomes at secondary schools. This has barely been studied in previous research and sometimes with contradictory results.

Self-efficacy is a key personal variable of Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) Bandura's ( 1986 ), defined as “an individual's belief in his or her own ability to organize and implement action to produce the desired achievements and results” (Bandura, 1997 , p. 3). Educational researchers have paid plenty of attention to this construct (see Michaelides, 2008 , for a review). Prior studies have provided strong evidence that self-efficacy is a positive predictor of performance outcomes in different subjects (Schunk et al., 2008 ; Usher and Pajares, 2008 ). For instance, Usher and Pajares ( 2008 , p. 751) argued that self-efficacy “predicts students' academic achievement across academic areas and levels.” Despite there being considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, studies that have explored the motivational mechanism which mediates self-efficacy–achievement relationship are scarce, and are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement, and will allow instructional actions and programs to improve academic achievement to be designed. One of the most solid proposals that integrate these variables is the social cognitive Expectancy-Value Model (E-VM) of achievement motivation, created by Eccles and her colleagues (Eccles et al., 1983 ; Wigfield and Eccles, 1992 , 2000 ) based on Atkinson's ( 1964 ) expectancy-value model. This complex model includes multiple connections and components that can be classified into three main blocks/categories of variables, arranged in the following sequential order: social world, cognitive processes, and motivational beliefs. All these blocks of variables act directly or indirectly as predictors of students' achievement behavior, persistence, and choice. Centered on motivational beliefs, this model assumes that; first, expectancies for success (achievement expectancy is considered a component of expectancy for success) and subjective task values are directly related to achievement, task choices and persistence; and second, expectancies and task value are assumed to be influenced by individuals' goals and self-schemata. Self-efficacy or personal beliefs of competence is/are considered a salient aspect of self-schemata. Another model that shares similarities with E-VM is the Educational Situation Quality Model (Doménech, 2006 , 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ; MOCSE is the acronym in Spanish) because: (a) both models are rooted in the social cognitive perspective of motivation; (b) they emphasize the important role that expectancy-value variables play in predicting students outcomes; (c) self-beliefs constructs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, self-confidence, etc.) are considered important antecedents of expectancy-value variables.

Based on the aforementioned theoretical frameworks, the purpose of this study is to test the validity of a structural model by integrating self-efficacy (adolescent students' self-belief) and expectancy and value constructs into predicting and explaining academic achievement and course satisfaction at secondary school. To examine how these motivational beliefs are related and affect such important students outcomes, they are important to not only design actions and programs to improve teacher effectiveness and students' academic results, but to also contribute to clarify the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value variables in predicting students outcomes, whose results available to date are limited and sometimes contradictory (Williams, 2010 ).

The term “outcomes” may refer to cognitive and emotional variables. Regarding cognitive variables, learning achievements are considered the most important. As regards emotional variables, satisfaction with a course is an important outcome since it influences students' decisions to continue with or drop out of a course (Levy, 2007 ). Satisfaction is also an important requirement for successful learning (Sinclaire, 2014 ). The majority of the considered students' outcomes have to do with cognitive variables such as academic achievement (e.g., grades, test scores, etc.) or learning strategies. In the current study we have decided to include, besides academic achievement, an emotional dependent variable that has been less studied by authors in this tradition, such as, course satisfaction. Academic achievement and course satisfaction are considered two complementary learning outcome as the two face of the same coin. Teachers are interested in knowing not only if their student's progress, but also if they are satisfied with the T–L process followed. Both constructs are important indicators of the quality of the teaching-learning (T–L) process. Therefore, we believe that it would be interesting to test if the selected motivational variables differed in predicting and explaining both academic achievement and students' course satisfaction.

Regarding the relationship between self-efficacy and student satisfaction, Pajares and Schunk ( 2001 ) stated that a strong sense of efficacy enhances human well-being; for instance, self-efficacy beliefs influence the amount of stress and anxiety that people experience as they engage in an activity (Pajares and Miller, 1994 ), and probably when students engage in a course. Self-efficacy also predicts course satisfaction in traditional face-to face classrooms (Bandura, 1997 ). Although there is empirical evidence to support the positive effects of self-efficacy beliefs on students' well-being and course satisfaction (DeWitz and Walsh, 2002 ), the motivational mechanisms that mediate the self-efficacy–students satisfaction relationship is still a problem to be solved. Very few studies have centered on examining the mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy–students' course satisfaction relationship, and are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' course satisfaction. These findings could provide important clues to promote student satisfaction. Student satisfaction is related to improved academic performance and the decision to take additional classes (Booker and Rebman, 2005 ). Moreover, satisfaction at school is fundamental for the judgments that students make of their own general well-being (Cummins and Tomyn, 2011 ).

Students' expectancy-value beliefs

The Expectancy-value theory is grounded in the social cognitive perspective of motivation. Psychologists in this tradition argue that individuals' choice, persistence, and vigor expended in performance can be predicted and explained basically by expectations of achievement and the value attributed to a task; i.e., by their beliefs about how well they will do in the task and the extent to which they value the task (Atkinson, 1957 ; Wigfield and Eccles, 1992 ; Wigfield, 1994 ). Apart from the components noted above, some theorists from this tradition have introduced a third construct related to the feelings experienced by students when they do a task (Pintrich and De Groot, 1990 ). We name this third construct “process expectancy.” In the course/subject matter context, we understood process expectancy to be the positive feelings that students expect to experience in their interaction with their teacher during the course (How will I feel studying this subject?). Indeed experience tells us that no-one starts something that is not worthwhile or when expectations of success are very poor because completing the task in such circumstances is considered a waste of time. Finally, nobody starts a task if they do not expect be feel well during the performance process. Hence these beliefs are considered three important indicators of students' motivation (Pintrich and De Groot, 1990 ), which specify some underlying motivational mechanisms that lead to the initiation and maintenance of action (Pintrich and Schunk, 1996 ). Centered on a course-subject and based on the above arguments, we herein used three types of beliefs that can make adolescent students decide on striving to learn a subject or not: (a) the subject value (What value does this subject have for me?), (b) the achievement expectancy (Will I be able to pass this subject?), and (c) process expectancy (How will I feel studying this subject?).

The modern Expectancy-value theory (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002 ; Eccles, 2009 ) distinguishes four task-value components: attainment value, intrinsic value, utility value and cost. For the present study, which centered on a course subject, we used extrinsic value (which encompasses utility, importance, and interestingness) and cost-benefit components to assess the subject matter value. Eccles and Wigfield ( 2002 ) identified cost as a critical component of value, which was conceptualized as a negative determinant in engaging a task due, for instance, to performance anxiety and fear of failure, and to the amount of effort needed to succeed (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002 ). However, when we centered on a course, we understood that being involved in a specific subject depends not only on the time and effort invested, but also on the benefits (e.g., in terms of results, reinforcements, enjoyment, etc.) that students can obtain. In short, the subject value items employed in this work refer to: (a) the extrinsic subject value, i.e., the perceived utility, importance, and interestingness of the subject (What value does this subject have for me?); (b) the expected cost-benefit relationship to pass the subject (Will it be worth the time and effort that I will have to invest to pass the subject?).

Students' expectancy-value beliefs may have been generated before classes began, from previous experiences, or may arise on the first days of class when students meet the teacher and find out about the study syllabus, evaluation requirements, teacher methodology, etc. (Doménech, 2006 , 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ). This means that these beliefs can be evaluated at the beginning of the course after some days/week of class.

The mediator role of expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and the achievement/satisfaction relationship

Regarding the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs.

When students face a new academic task, they ask themselves “Can I perform this task?” (self-efficacy) and “Why should I do this task?” (task value). If their answer to the first question is “yes,” they proceed to the next question (Keskin, 2014 ). This reasoning suggests that self-efficacy is considered a predictor of task value, and not vice versa. Previous studies have demonstrated not only a positive relationship between both constructs (Bong, 2001 ; Seo and Taherbhai, 2009 ), but also that self-efficacy is a direct predictor of task value (Kozanitis et al., 2007 ; Azar et al., 2010 ; Keskin, 2014 ).

Prior research has also revealed significant and substantial direct effects of students' self-efficacy on academic expectations (Chemers et al., 2001 ; Lent et al., 2008 ). According to these authors, students with high self-efficacy have greater academic expectations and display better academic performance that with low self-efficacy. These findings are consistent with what Bandura's postulated Bandura's ( 1997 ) when he argued that self-efficacy is causally prior to outcome expectancy as the results that individuals anticipate depend mainly on their judgments of how well they would be able to perform in a given situation (Bandura, 1997 ). Therefore, it is assumed that self-efficacy (defined as the perceived capability to perform a given behavior) causally influences expected outcomes of behavior, but not vice versa.

In short, as regards the relationship between self-efficacy and the expectancy-value variables, the above-described findings support the notion that competence beliefs may drive students' expectations and task/subject values in the school context. However, more studies are needed to understand the connections between students' self-beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, etc.) and expectancy-value variables.

Regarding the relationship between expectancy-value beliefs and achievement

Prior research has provided empirical evidence which indicates that expectancies and task-values are related to academic choices and achievement in specific domains, such as mathematics (Marsh and Yeung, 1997 ; Spinath et al., 2004 ) and language arts (Spinath et al., 2004 ). Recent cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have found that expectancy beliefs strongly influence achievement, whereas subject value considerably impacts choice, effort and persistence (Nagengast et al., 2011 ; Gasco and Villarroel, 2014 ; Guo et al., 2015 ).

Regarding the relationship between expectancy-value beliefs and satisfaction

Less is known about how students' expectancy-value beliefs relate to emotional outcomes, such as student satisfaction. Despite the findings being limited, they seem to support that satisfaction is well explained by task value (Artino, 2008 ; Diep et al., 2016 ) and by grade expectancies (Svanum and Aigner, 2011 ). Nonetheless, most authors highlight the teacher role and teacher–student interaction (Wu et al., 2010 ) in relation to instructional and emotional supports as the main responsible factors of students' course satisfaction. Accordingly, process expectations, specifically related to the feelings that student experience during their interaction with the teacher, may play the most salient role to explain students' satisfaction. However, the process expectation formed by students can be influenced, in turn, by self-efficacy beliefs. Students with strong self-efficacy beliefs visualize success scenarios, which provide supportive resources, and guidance for performance (Bandura, 1993 ). As a result, these students tend to experience more satisfaction with the teaching process than the students with low self-efficacy.

Finally, taken all de variables simultaneously, structural models tested in previous studies, based on the expectancy-value theory, provide additional and important evidence to support the mediator role played by motivational expectancy-value variables in the relationship between students' self-beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy, self-concept, self-esteem, etc.) and students outcomes. For example, the study conducted by Doménech-Betoret et al. ( 2014 ) in the university context revealed that students' academic self-efficacy had a significant and direct effect on achievement expectations, enjoyable learning expectations and expected dedication (cost) and, in turn, achievement expectation had a significant and direct effect on avoidance strategies (students' outcomes). In addition, subject value had a significant and direct effect on avoidance strategies (students' outcomes). In a similar vein, the study conducted by Bong et al. ( 2012 ) in the school context found that the task value and test anxiety significantly mediated the relationships of self-efficacy to achievement.

Based on the aforementioned empirical evidence, it is plausible to assume that the motivators beliefs which derive from the Expectancy-value theory may mediate the relationship between self-efficacy and learning outcomes; e.g., academic achievement and student satisfaction.

Objectives and hypotheses

According to the aforementioned rationale, the main aim of this study was to examine if the motivational beliefs derived from the Expectancy-value theory play a mediator role between academic self-efficacy and learning outcomes (achievement and satisfaction). At the same time, another aim was to identify some of the motivational processes through which students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement and satisfaction (see Figure ​ Figure1). 1 ). Accordingly, we hypothesized that expectancy-value beliefs would have a direct effect on academic achievement and satisfaction, whereas academic self-efficacy would have an indirect effect on academic achievement and satisfaction through expectancy-value variables. In other words, we predicted that expectancy-value variables would play a mediator role between self-efficacy and achievement (H1), and between self-efficacy and satisfaction (H2). The hypothesized connections were addressed and tested by the Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) procedure with the EQS program (Bentler, 2006 ). Self-efficacy and expectancy-value variables were all measured in the first academic term after some weeks of class, and student achievement and satisfaction were all measured in the third and last terms of the course. This study can provide new data to improve the motivational theories that integrate self-efficacy, expectancy, and value constructs in the educational setting context, focused on a subject matter as a unit of analysis. It may also help identify the motivational connections that mediate between academic self-efficacy and students' achievement/satisfaction. Important implications for educational practice may derive from these findings since they can provide valuable information to design instructional actions and programs that can improve student achievement and satisfaction. Student achievement and satisfaction are two of the most important learning outcomes, and are also considered key indicators of education quality.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-08-01193-g0001.jpg

Grafical representation of the study. * Underlying Motivational Mechanisms (UMM) are partially operationalized by the following questions: What value does this subject have for me?, How much time and effort will I invest to pass the subject?, Will I be successful in this subject?, and How will I feel studying this subject?.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure.

The sample consisted of 797 Spanish secondary education students from 36 classes with different subjects, of whom 404 were male (50.7%) and 393 were female (49.3%), and they were aged between 12 and 17 years. Most of their teachers ( N = 23, 63.88%) also participated in the study, of whom 11 were males (average experience = 29 years) and 12 were females (average experience = 32 years). About 80% of the participating students were Spanish, while the parents of the rest had come from other counties (the majority from Romania, Ecuador, and Morocco) to Spain as emigrants some years ago. One private and two state secondary schools located in east Spain took part in this study, which was carried out at the first four levels of compulsory secondary education: 1st ESO (12–13 years old), 2nd ESO (13–14 years old), 3rd ESO (14–15 years old), and 4th ESO (16–17 years old; ESO is the Spanish acronym for Educación Secundaria Obligatoria—Compulsory Secondary Education). Table ​ Table1 1 displays sample distribution according to levels of education and centers. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Regional Valencian Government, Spain. Consent for students to participate was required from students' parents or legal tutors. Confidentiality and personal data protection were guaranteed in accordance with current Spanish law.

Characteristics and sample distribution according to courses and centers.

12–13NSLLPrivate3735724
12–13IESLPPublic3784853223811
13–14IESFRPublic16911110710048721
Students according to level of education243230192132Total = 797Total = 36

The scales used to measure the variables considered herein have been reviewed and refined in previous studies (Doménech, 2006 , 2012 , 2013 ; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ). As most had been designed for university students, they had to be adapted to secondary education in order to use them herein. The scales that referred to self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were administered at time 1 (halfway through the first term), and student satisfaction and achievement were measured at time 2 (halfway through the third term). See Table ​ Table2 2 for item examples.

Summary of the factor analysis, internal consistency and item example of the scales.

) .
72562.17
   F1: Study techniques42.880.8110.080.77“How good are you at making summaries to help you study?”
   F2: Planning and organization32.580.839.560.82“How well do you plan your work and study?”
   F3: Team work skills43.210.759.410.74“How well do you cope with teamwork with colleagues?”
   F4: Coping with new technologies43.180.698.870.73“How good are you at looking for information on the Internet for your classwork?”
   F5: Memorization capacity32.860.808.550.74“How well do you memorize what you study for an exam?”
   F6: Oral and writing communication42.920.878.360.61“How well do you express what you want to say in writing?”
   F7: Coping with exam situations and stress32.670.927.310.70“How do you cope in exam situations?”
41377.06
   F1: Cost expectancy42.371.0519.430.90“Will the time and effort you must invest to pass this subject be too much according to the importance you attach to this subject?”
   F2: Achievement expectancy32.900.9514.500.85“Do you think you will be able to obtain good marks for this subject?”
   F3: Process expectancy33.240.8914.250.83“Do you think you will feel well treated by the teacher during the course?”
   F4: Subject value33.020.8413.520.79“How useful is this subject for you?”
   F1: Students' satisfaction of the Teaching Process53.050.9256.050.81“Are you satisfied with the help and guidelines the teacher provided to complete your classwork and tasks?”
Student' marks ranged from 1 (minimum) to 10 (maximum)

Students' general academic self-efficacy scale (25 items, α = 0.86)

This scale is based on the original scales created by Bandura ( 1990 ) and by Pastorelli et al. ( 2001 ). This scale was used to assess students' self-perception of how competent they were in the academic field. Students indicated their level of agreement within the 1 (very bad) to 4 (very good) range.

Expectancy-value scale (13 items, α = 0.78)

This scale comprises 13 items and was designed to measure expectancy-value constructs at the beginning of the teaching-learning process. It was structured and designed according to the Motivational Theory proposed by Pintrich ( 1989 ) and Pintrich and De Groot ( 1990 ). Students indicated their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale within the 1 (I am absolutely unconvinced) to 5 (I am absolutely convinced) range.

Satisfaction of the teaching process scale (5 items, α = 0.81)

The original scale was designed by Doménech ( 2011 , 2012 ) to assess university students' satisfaction with the teaching process followed in the classroom for a specific subject matter. The scale used herein was composed of five items and is a short version of the original teaching process scale adapted to secondary education. Students indicated their level of satisfaction with the teaching process, and opinions were viewed on a 4-point Likert scale within the 1 (unsatisfied) to 5 (very satisfy) range. Finally, student achievement was measured with the marks obtained by students for the first and second academic terms. The mark expected for the third term was also required. Achievement scores ranged from 1 (minimum) to 10 (maximum).

Statistical analyses

The hypothesized connections were tested by the SEM procedure. The ML and ML robust method of estimation (if the assumption of multivariate normal distribution was violated), developed by Satorra and Bentler ( 1988 , 1994 ), was used with the EQS program (Bentler, 2006 ) to calculate the fit indices of the hypothesized models. Since the Chi-square test is sensitive to sample size, using relative fit indices like CFI, the NNFI, and RMSEA is highly recommended (Bentler, 1990 ). Values below 0.05 for RMSEA indicate a good fit, whereas values up to 0.08 denote an unacceptable fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1993 ). NNFI- and CFI-values above 0.90 (Hoyle, 1995 ), or even 0.95 (Hu and Bentler, 1999 ), were fixed as the cutting-off point.

Validity of the measurement model of latent variables

Hypothesized covariance structure models represent only approaches of reality because the obtained indices may be driven by the sample characteristics on which the model was tested (Cudeck and Browne, 1983 ). One approach to mitigate this limitation is to employ the cross-validation strategy (Byrne, 2012 ). To apply this strategy, the total sample ( N = 797) was randomly split into two equivalent subsamples (the calibration sample and the validation sample), following the recommendations of Cudeck and Browne ( 1983 ). First, with subsample 1 ( n = 399), a separate explorative factor analysis (EFA), using the principal component method with varimax rotation, was conducted on all the scales to estimate their factorial structure. Second, by taking the factors extracted in the EFA as the observational variables, a separate confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed with subsample 2 ( n = 398) to test the goodness of fit and stability of the measurement models of these scales. These two-handed factorial analyses (exploratory and confirmatory) approach provide strong evidence for the reliability of the factors used as latent variables, and improve the validity of the measurement model (Cudeck and Browne, 1983 ). Finally, the total sample ( N = 797) was then used to examine the structural model; i.e., the relationships among the latent variables (see Table ​ Table2 2 for details). When data analyses were performed, the initial sample slightly reduced because 23 students did not complete the entire scales. Missing values were not calculated given the large number of participants.

Students' general academic self-efficacy scale (25 items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). Seven factors that corresponded to the seven academic skills included on the scale were extracted, which accounted for 62.17% of total variance. Cronbach's alpha values ranged between 0.82 (maximum) and 0.61 (minimum).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Sample 2). The fit indices values obtained using the ML (χ 2 = 573.459; p = 0.000, d.f . = 254; χ 2 / d.f . = 2.257; NFI = 0.831; NNFI = 0.878; CFI = 0.897; GFI = 0.892; AGFI = 0.861; RMSEA = 0.056) and ML Robust (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ 2 = 478.366; p = 0.000, d.f . = 254; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.883; NFI = 0.823; NNFI = 0.890; CFI = 0.907; IFI = 0.909; MFI = 0.754; RMSEA = 0.047) estimation methods indicated that the model fitted the data.

Expectancy-value scale (13 items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). Four factors, which corresponded to the four scale constructs, were extracted, and accounted for 77.06% of total variance. Cronbach's alpha values ranged between 0.90 (maximum) and 0.79 (minimum).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Sample 2). The fit indices values obtained using the ML (χ 2 = 136.238; p = 0.000, d.f . = 94; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.449; NFI = 0.963; NNFI = 0.985; CFI = 0.988; GFI = 0.960; AGFI = 0.942; RMSEA = 0.034) and ML Robust (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ 2 = 117.663; p = 0.000, d.f . = 94; χ 2 / d.f . = 1.251; NFI = 0.963; NNFI = 0.990; CFI = 0.992; IFI = 0.992; MFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.025) estimation methods indicated that the model fitted the data.

Satisfaction with the teaching process scale (5 items)

Exploratory Factor Analysis (Sample 1). One factor referred to satisfaction with the teaching process (α = 0.81), and was extracted and accounted for 56.05% of total variance. The confirmatory factorial analysis (Sample 2) was not applicable because only one factor was extracted.

Procedure for testing mediation

The structural equation analysis was carried out with the whole sample to firstly test the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between the self-efficacy–achievement relationship, and secondly the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between the self-efficacy–satisfaction relationship. The procedure followed to test the mediation effect of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and achievement, and also between self-efficacy and satisfaction, was conducted in two steps: first by testing the significant direct effects of latent variable self-efficacy on latent variables achievement (M1A model) and satisfaction (M1S model); second by testing the mediated role of the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs on the self-efficacy–achievement relationship (M2A), and also on the efficacy–satisfaction relationship (M2S). In this case we considered the direct and indirect effects between self-efficacy and achievement/satisfaction simultaneously.

Testing the mediation effect of expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and achievement (H1)

The M1A model was first tested (direct effects) for the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and achievement. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 194.52; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 34; NNFI = 0.93; CFI = 0.95; GFI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.078) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 173.19; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 34; NNFI = 0.93; CFI = 0.95; RMSEA = 0.073) indicated that the model satisfactorily fitted the data. According to the data, academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on achievement. So this prerequisite for mediation to exist was met (Baron and Kenny, 1986 ).

Next the mediated model M2A was tested. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 329.77; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 74; NNFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.94; GFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.067) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 293.87; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 74; NNFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.062) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, latent variable academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on expectancy-value beliefs, which, in turn, had a significant effect on achievement. On the contrary, the path between academic self-efficacy and achievement was not significant. See Figure ​ Figure2 2 for details.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-08-01193-g0002.jpg

M2A model (direct and indirect effects). Relationship among students' academic self-efficacy, expectancy-value beliefs, and achievement. The structural configuration and standardized coefficients of the model are displayed. * Significant ( p < 0.05), n.s., not significant.

Testing the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between the self-efficacy–satisfaction relationship (H2)

The M1S model was first tested (direct effects) for the mediation role of the expectancy-value beliefs between self-efficacy and satisfaction. The model was optimized when a covariance between two variable errors (E10–E12) from the self-efficacy latent variable was introduced, following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the model was tested again. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 197.88; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 52; NNFI = 0.926; CFI = 0.942; GFI = 0.959; RMSEA = 0.060) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 163.57; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 52; NNFI = 0.931; CFI = 0.946; RMSEA = 0.053) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, Academic Self-Efficacy had a significant effect on teaching process satisfaction. So this prerequisite for mediation to exist was met (Baron and Kenny, 1986 ).

Second the mediated model M2S was tested. The model was optimized when a covariance between two variable errors (E10–E12) from the self-efficacy latent variable was introduced, following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the model was tested again. The fit indices values obtained by the ML method (χ 2 = 399.86; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 100; NNFI = 0.891; CFI = 0.909; GFI = 0.938; RMSEA = 0.062) and the ML Robust method of estimation (χ 2 = 343.17; p = 0.0000, d.f . = 100; NNFI = 0.894; CFI = 0.912; RMSEA = 0.056) indicated that the model fitted the data well. According to the data, the latent variable academic self-efficacy had a significant effect on expectancy-value beliefs which, in turn, had a significant effect on teaching satisfaction. On the contrary, the path between academic self-efficacy and teaching satisfaction was not significant. See Figure ​ Figure3 3 for details.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-08-01193-g0003.jpg

M2S model (direct and indirect effects). Relationship among students' academic self-efficacy, expectancy-value beliefs, and teaching process satisfaction. The structural configuration and standardized coefficients of the optimized model are displayed. * Significant ( p < 0.05), n.s., not significant.

The description and fit indices of the tested models are provided in Table ​ Table3, 3 , which summarizes the structural equation analyses results.

Fit indices of the tested models ( N = 797).

ML method194.520.000340.930.950.920.078
ML Robust method173.190.000340.930.950.073
ML method329.770.000740.920.940.940.067
ML Robust method293.870.000740.920.940.062
ML method197.880.000520.920.940.960.060
ML Robust method163.570.000520.930.940.053
ML method399.860.0001000.890.910.930.062
ML Robust Method343.170.0001000.890.910.056

Based on a socio-cognitive perspective of motivation, this study examines; first, the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement; second, the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction with the teaching process. Achievement and satisfaction were considered dependent variables in two separate models as indicators of teaching practice quality.

For the mediator role played by the expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement, and following the recommendation by Baron and Kenny ( 1986 ) for testing mediation, the prediction capacity of students' academic self-efficacy on student achievement was examined first by testing the M1A model; second the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement was examined by testing the M2A model. According to the M1A model, the structural analyses indicated a direct, positive and significant effect of the latent variable academic self-efficacy on achievement. According to the M2A model, the obtained fit indices supported the hypothesized connections. This means that the expectancy-value beliefs mediated the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student achievement. These results indicated that students' academic self-efficacy affects student achievement, but only indirectly; i.e., by fulfilling the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs.

For the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction with the teaching process, and following the recommendation by Baron and Kenny ( 1986 ) for testing mediation, the prediction capacity of students' academic self-efficacy on student satisfaction was examined first by testing the M1S model; second the mediator role played by expectancy-value beliefs in the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction was examined by testing the M2S model.

In accordance with the M1S model, the structural analyses indicated a positive and significant direct effect of students' academic self-efficacy on the latent variable teaching process. According to the M2S model, the obtained fit indices supported the hypothesized connections. This means that the expectancy-value beliefs mediated the relationship between students' academic self-efficacy and student satisfaction. These results suggested that students' academic self-efficacy affects student satisfaction, but only indirectly; i.e., by fulfilling the latent variable expectancy-value beliefs.

Given the remarkable variance explained in both models (M2A and M2S), it can be stated that general academic self-efficacy has a strong effect on expectancy-value beliefs. These findings indicated that the level of activation and quality of students' expectancy-value beliefs during the first weeks of the teaching-learning process (after some weeks attending class) depended to a great extent on the evaluation that students made of their own academic skills/capabilities (self-beliefs); e.g., study techniques, planning study, team work skills, coping with new technologies, memorization capacity, oral and written communication, and coping with exam situations. In the light of the obtained results, academic self-efficacy can be considered an important internal source of motivation that is capable of activating students' motivation in the first stage of the behavioral process; i.e., academic self-efficacy contributes to a great extent to activate student students' motivation from the first weeks of the teaching-learning process undertaken with a specific subject. Therefore, it is important to take into account students' academic self-efficacy when they face a new educational setting. These findings are similar to others obtained in previous research. Thus, the study conducted by Doménech-Betoret et al. ( 2014 ) revealed the key role played by academic self-efficacy in explaining students' expectations (achievement expectations, enjoyable learning expectations, and expected dedication according to the subject value). In a similar vein, the structural model tested by Bong et al. ( 2012 ) revealed that self-beliefs (self-efficacy and self-concept) are good predictors of task value.

Expectancy-value beliefs had a direct positive and significant effect on student achievement/satisfaction. These findings suggested that expectancy-value beliefs (Achievement expectations, Value of the subject matter, Process expectations with the teacher, Expected cost to pass the subject), which were evaluated some weeks after the course began, would be capable of satisfactorily explaining and predicting student achievement and their degree of satisfaction with the teaching process followed with a specific subject matter. The observational variables with higher loadings (Achievement expectations, Value of the subject and Satisfaction expectations with the process) on the latent factor expectancy-value beliefs suggested that these motivational variables were the most important predictors of student achievement and satisfaction. These findings fall in line with previous studies that used the variables from the Expectation-value theory (Guo et al., 2015 ). The structural model tested by these authors evidenced that Math self-concept (construct used to assess students' expectancy of success) and the Math utility value had a significant and direct effect on students' academic achievement and educational aspirations.

All these findings moved in the expected direction. Academic self-efficacy, considered a general domain variable (Boekaerts, 1999 ), predicted and explained students' specific expectancy-value beliefs in connection with a specific educational setting. In turn, these specific expectancy-value beliefs predicted/explained students' outcomes (academic achievement and satisfaction). These results were coherent with what Bandura postulated when claiming that specific measures of beliefs were more closely related to behavior (Bandura, 1997 ).

Based on the obtained results, the following conclusions can be drawn:

  • Expectancy-value beliefs, understood as the anticipatory previsions and forecasts that students make in an attempt to anticipate their actions, emotions and results in a new educational situation, were well measured and operationalized by the four motivational selected factors that derived from the Expectancy-value theory: Value of the subject, achievement expectancy, satisfaction expectancy with the process, and the expected cost to pass the subject.
  • Students' expectancy-value beliefs, generated/activated during the first weeks of the teaching-learning process, were well explained by the perception or idea that students form about their own basic academic skills. These findings also fall in line with previous studies (Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014 ).
  • Four motivational variables, which mediate the relationship between academic self-efficacy and students' achievement/satisfaction, were identified. These findings fall in line with previous studies, which examined the mediator role of motivational variables in the relationship between self-efficacy and achievement (Bong et al., 2012 ).
  • These findings shed light to better understand the relationship between self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs in predicting students' outcomes in secondary education.
  • This study allows advances to be made in explaining students' emotional outcomes, such as course satisfaction, which has barely been studied in previous research in the expectancy-value theory context.
  • Important educational implications can be derived from the socio-cognitive perspective of motivation acquired from the results obtained to improve students' achievement and satisfaction from a preventive point of view.

Educational implications

Based on the results obtained in this study, the following educational implications can be made:

First, the expectancy-value beliefs generated during the first weeks of the course are capable of predicting student achievement and their satisfaction with the teaching process followed throughout the course. Therefore, we wish to stress the importance of making a diagnostic evaluation at the beginning of the course of secondary students' expectancy-value beliefs in order to: (a) detect possible shortcomings that students may present in relation to students' expectancy-value beliefs formed at the beginning of the course, after some days of class; (b) design an action plan to overcome or improve these shortcomings.

Second, the obtained results provide evidence that general academic self-efficacy is capable of explaining to a great extent the expectancy-value beliefs formed by secondary students about a specific subject some days after the course starts. Therefore teachers should also take it into account at the very beginning of the course. Accordingly, implementing actions and programs at schools is recommended to improve students' academic skills to, in turn, improve academic self-efficacy. These programs should include a variety of components that fall in line with the sources of self-efficacy beliefs proposed by Bandura ( 1997 ) in academic contexts. According to Bandura ( 1997 ), self-efficacy beliefs are developed when individuals interpret information from four major sources, such as mastery experience, vicarious experience of observing others, social persuasions that students receive from others, and emotional and psychological states. In the school context, mastery experience refers to the way students interpret and evaluate obtained results, and self-beliefs of competence are revised and created according to these interpretations. Accordingly, teachers should provide instructional scenarios in which students are able to succeed in challenging tasks. Students' judgments of competence are also created by vicarious experience; i.e., by evaluating their capabilities in relation to other students' performance. Another source of self-efficacy is the social persuasions that students receive from others. Accordingly, a supportive message from parents and teachers is important to empower students' self-confidence. Finally, students' self-efficacy is created by their emotional and psychological states as students tend to interpret negative psychological states (stress, anxiety, bad mood, depression, etc.) as evidence for lack of skills, and positive psychological states as indicators of personal competence. Accordingly, promoting students well-being and reducing negative emotional states strengthen students' self-efficacy. For more details about sources of self-efficacy, see the review by Usher and Pajares ( 2008 ). In short, the actions and programs that aim to develop students' self-efficacy should be based on these four sources.

We defend the notion of quality education based on a preventive view. Accordingly, we suggest secondary school teachers taking specific actions on the first days of the T–L process to improve adolescent students' beliefs as regards academic self-efficacy (self-beliefs), achievement expectancy, process expectancy and subject value.

  • It is important for teachers to strive to transmit the idea that all the students in class are capable of passing the subject matter. This relates with students' psychological need of competence (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).
  • From the very beginning of the course, improve students' perception of their own capacity, specifically the general academic skills required to improve progress made at school; e.g., taking actions to bridge some basic gaps in training that some students may still have from former courses, and are necessary to make progress in the subject; or evaluate and recognize the progress made by students since the evaluative feedback that students receive contribute to develop their competence. This point relates with students' psychological need of competence (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).
  • Explain to students the value of the subject matter when presenting the subject matter syllabus, and also throughout the course. Inform students about the importance and usefulness of the subject matter (present or future) at the personal, academic and professional levels.
  • From the very beginning of the course, promote and take care of the teacher–students interpersonal relationship; e.g., show closeness, respect, and empathy with students throughout the course. This relates with students' psychological need of relatedness (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ).

Limitations and suggestions for future research

Although the results obtained herein are satisfactory, some limitations and suggestions for future research should be pointed out.

First, the results were obtained from schools located in a specific socio-cultural context. Thus, replicating this study in other educational and cultural contexts is recommended to generalize the findings.

Second, extending the tested model should be considered first by introducing other types of self-efficacy, such as metacognitive self-efficacy (Moores et al., 2006 ) or emotion regulation self-efficacy (Gross and John, 2003 ); second by including other motivational variables as mediators. Regarding the motivational process, Bandura ( 1986 ) distinguished three types of cognitive motivators: (a) causal attributions; (b) outcomes expectations; (c) goals, whose corresponding theories are Attribution theory, Expectancy-value theory, and Goal theory. Accordingly, including new variables as mediators in future research, such as, goal orientation (Pintrich, 2000 ) or achievement goals, would be interesting (Liem et al., 2008 ).

Third, although academic self-efficacy and expectancy-value beliefs were measured in the same data collection wave, we provide enough evidence and theoretical support to consider general academic self-efficacy as an antecedent of the students' expectancy-value belief generated in the classroom context.

Fourth, according to Wigfield and Cambria ( 2010 ), most of the measures used by researchers to assess motivational beliefs are student self-report measures. However, self-report measures can be problematic, especially for young children or for students who state that school is not important to them. Consequently, we wish to emphasize the importance of combining quantitative and qualitative methods to reduce biases and to obtain more complete information about students' belief.

Ethics statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Regional Government of Valencia, Spain, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author contributions

All authors made substantial contributions to design the work, analysis and interpretation of the data, drafting and revising the work, final approval of the version to be published, and finally agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in insuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any aspects of the work were appropriately investigated and resolved (FD, LA, and AG).

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Artino A. R. (2008). Motivational beliefs and perceptions of instructional quality: predicting satisfaction with online training . J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 24 , 260–270. 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00258.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Atkinson J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior . Psychol. Rev . 64 ( 6 Pt 1 ), 359–372. 10.1037/h0043445 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Atkinson J. W. (1964). An Introduction to Motivation . Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Azar H. K., Lavasani M. G., Malahmadi E., Amani J. (2010). The role of self- efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning approaches and mathematics achievement . Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 5 , 942–947. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.214 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and action: A Social Cognitive Theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. (1990). Mechanisms of moral disengagement , in Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies, Theologies, States of mind , ed Reich W. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ), 161–191. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning . Educ. Psychol. 28 , 117–148. 10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control . New York, NY: Freeman. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baron R. M., Kenny D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychology research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51 , 1173–1182. 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bentler P. M. (1990). Fit indexes, langrage multipliers, constraint changes and incomplete data in structural models . Multivariate Behav. Res. 25 , 163–172. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_3 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bentler P. M. (2006). EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual . Ecino, CA: Multivarite Sotware Inc. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boekaerts M. (1999). Motivated learning: studying student situation transactional units . Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 14 , 41–55. 10.1007/BF03173110 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bong M. (2001). Between- and within-domain relations of academic motivation among middle and high school students: self-efficacy, task-value, and achievement goals . J. Educ. Psychol. 93 , 23–34. 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.23 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bong M., Cho C., Ahn H. S., Kim H. J. (2012). Comparison of self-beliefs for predicting student motivation and achievement . J. Educ. Res. 105 , 336–352. 10.1080/00220671.2011.627401 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Booker Q. E., Rebman C. E. (2005). E-Student retention: factors affecting customer loyalty for online program success . Issues Inform. Syst. 6 , 183–189. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Browne M. W., Cudeck R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit , in Testing Structural Equation Models , eds Bollen K. A., Long J. S. (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; ), 136–162. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Byrne B. (2012). Structural Equation Modeling with Mplus. Basic Concepts, Apllications and Programing . New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chemers M. M., Hu L., Garcia B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year college student performance and adjustment . J. Educ. Psychol. 93 , 55–64. 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.55 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cudeck R., Browne M. W. (1983). Cross-validation of covariance structures . Multivariate Behav. Res. 18 , 147–167. 10.1207/s15327906mbr1802_2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cummins R., Tomyn A. (2011). The subjective well being of high-school students: validating the personal wellbeing index-school children . Soc. Indic. Res. 101 , 405–418. 10.1007/s11205-010-9668-6 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuit: human needs and the self-determination of behavior . Psychol. Inq. 11 , 227–268. 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deci E., Ryan R. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • DeWitz S. J., Walsh W. B. (2002). Self-efficacy and college student satisfaction . J. Career Assess. 10 , 315–326. 10.1177/10672702010003003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diep A. N., Zhu Ch., Struyven K., Blieck Y. (2016). Who and What contributes to student satisfaction in different blended learning modalities? Br. J. Educ. Technol. 48 , 473–489. 10.1111/bjet.12431 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doménech F. (2006). Testing an instructional model in a university educational setting from the student's perspective . Learn. Instr. 16 , 450–466. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doménech F. (2011). Evaluar e Investigar en la Situación Educativa Universitaria. Un Nuevo Enfoque Desde el Espacio Europeo de Educación Superior. [Evaluate and Investigate in the University Educational Setting. A New Approach from the Higher European Area]. Publicacions de la Universitat Jaume I, Universitas; 34. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doménech F. (2012). Psicología Educativa: Su Aplicación al Contexto de la Clase [Educational Psychology: its Application in the Classroom Context] , Vol. 13 Castellón: Publicaciones de la Universitat Jaume I. Col·lecció Psique. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doménech F. (2013). An instructional model for guiding reflection and research in the classroom: the educational situation quality model . Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 11 , 239–260. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doménech-Betoret F., Gómez-Artiga A., Lloret-Segura S. (2014). Personal variables, motivation and avoidance learning strategies in undergraduate students . Learn. Individ. Differ. 35 , 122–129. 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.06.007 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eccles J. (2009). Who am i and what am i going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action . Educ. Psychol. 44 , 78–89. 10.1080/00461520902832368 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eccles J. S., Adler T. F., Futterman R., Goff S. B., Kaczala C. M., Meece J. L., et al. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors , in Achievement and Achievement Motivation , ed Spence J. T. (San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman; ) 75–146. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eccles J. S., Wigfield A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53 , 109–132. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gasco J., Villarroel J. D. (2014). The motivation of secondary school students in mathematical word problem solving . Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 12 , 83–106. 10.14204/ejrep.32.13076 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gross J. J., John O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships and well-being . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85 , 348–362. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guo J., Marsh H. W., Parker P. D., Morin A. J. S., Yeung A. S. (2015). Achievement, motivation, and educational choices: a longitudinal study of expectancy and value using a multiplicative perspective . Dev. Psychol. 51 , 1163–1176. 10.1037/a0039440 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoyle R. H. (1995). Structural Equation Modeling . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu L., Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: coventional criteria versus new alternatives . Struct. Equ. Model. 6 , 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keskin H. K. (2014). A path analysis of metacognitive strategies in reading, self-efficacy and task value . Int. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. 4 , 798–808. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kozanitis A., Desbiens J.-F., Chouinard R. (2007). Perception of teacher support and reaction towards questioning: its relation to instrumental help-seeking and motivation to learn . Int. J. Teach. Learn. High. Educ. 19 , 238–250. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lent R. W., Sheu H. B., Singley D., Schmidt J. A., Schmidt L. C., Gloster C. S. (2008). Longitudinal relations of self-efficacy to outcome expectations, interests, and major choice goals in engineering students . J. Vocat. Behav. 73 , 328–335. 10.1016/j.jvb.2008.07.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levy Y. (2007). Comparing dropouts and persistence in e-learning courses . Comput. Educ. 48 , 185–204. 10.1016/j.compedu.2004.12.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liem A. D., Lau S., Nie Y. (2008). The role of self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning strategies, task disengagement, peer relationship, and achievement outcome . Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33 , 486–512. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.08.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marsh H. W., Yeung A. S. (1997). Causal effects of academic self-concept on academic achievement: structural equation models of longitudinal data . J. Educ. Psychol. 89 , 41–54. 10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.41 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michaelides M. P. (2008). Emerging themes from early research on self-efficacy beliefs in school mathematics . Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 6 , 219–234. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moores T. T., Chang J. C., Smith D. K. (2006). Clarifying the role of self-efficacy and metacognition as indicators of learning: construct development and test . Database Adv. Inform. Syst. 37 , 125–132. 10.1145/1161345.1161360 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nagengast B., Marsh H. W., Scalas L. F., Xu M. K., Hau K. T., Trautwein U. (2011). Who took the “x” out of expectancy-value theory? A psychological mystery, a substantive-methodological synergy, and a cross-national generalization . Psychol. Sci. 22 , 1058–1066. 10.1177/0956797611415540 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pajares F., Miller M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical problem solving: a path analysis . J. Educ. Psychol. 86 :193 10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.193 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pajares F., Schunk D. H. (2001). Self-beliefs and school success: self-efficacy, selfconcept, and school achievement , in Perception , eds Riding R., Rayner S. (London: Ablex Publishing; ), 239–266. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pastorelli C., Caprara G. V., Barbaranelli C., Rola J., Rozsa S., Bandura A. (2001). The structure of children's perceived self-efficacy: a cross-national study . Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 17 , 87–97. 10.1027/1015-5759.17.2.87 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pintrich P. L., Schunk D. H. (1996). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, and Applications . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pintrich P. R. (1989). The dynamic interplay of student motivation and cognition in the college classroom , in Advances in Motivation and Achievement , Vol. 6 , eds En Ames C., Maher M. L. (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; ), 117–160. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pintrich P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning , in Handbook of Self-regulation , eds Boekaerts M., Pintrich P. R., Zeidner M. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 452–502. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pintrich P. R., De Groot E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom performance . J. Educ. Psychol. 82 , 33–40. 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Satorra A., Bentler P. M. (1988). Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. ASA 1988 , in Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics, Section , (Alexandria, VA: American Statistical Association: ), 308–313. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Satorra A., Bentler P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis , in Latent variables analysis: Applications for Developmental Research , eds von Eye A., Clogg C. C. (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; ), 399–419. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schunk D. H., Pintrich P. R., Meece J. L. (2008). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research and Applications, 3rd Edn. , Upper saddle River, NJ: Merrill-Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seo D. C., Taherbhai H. (2009). Motivational beliefs and cognitive processes in mathematics achievement, analyzed in the context of cultural differences: a Korean elementary school example . Asia Pac. Educ. Rev. 10 , 193–203. 10.1007/s12564-009-9017-0 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sinclaire J. K. (2014). An empirical investigation of student satisfaction with college courses . Res. High. Educ. J. 22 , 1–21. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spinath B., Spinath F. M., Harlaar N., Plomin R. (2004). Predicting school achievement from general cognitive ability, self-perceived ability, and intrinsic value . Intelligence 34 , 363–374. 10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Svanum S., Aigner C. (2011). The influences of course effort, mastery and performance goals, grade expectancies, and earned course grades on student ratings of course satisfaction . Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 81 , 667–679. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02011.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Usher E. L., Pajares F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: critical review of the literature and future directions . Rev. Educ. Res. 78 , 751–796. 10.3102/0034654308321456 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wigfield A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: a developmental perspective . Educ. Psychol. Rev. 6 , 49–78. 10.1007/BF02209024 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wigfield A., Cambria J. (2010). Students' achievement values, goal orientations, and interest: definitions, development, and relations to achievement outcomes . Dev. Rev. 30 , 1–35. 10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wigfield A., Eccles J. S. (1992). The development of achievement task values: a theoretical analysis . Dev. Rev. 12 , 265–310. 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90011-P [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wigfield A., Eccles J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation . Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25 , 68–81. 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams D. M. (2010). Outcome expectancy and self-efficacy: theoretical implications of an unresolved contradiction . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14 , 417–425. 10.1177/1088868310368802 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wu H., Tennyson R. D., Hsia T. (2010). A study of of student satisfaction in a blended e-learning system environment . Comput. Educ. 55 , 155–164. 10.1016/j.compedu.2009.12.012 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Grab your spot at the free arXiv Accessibility Forum

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Computation and Language

Title: mutual reasoning makes smaller llms stronger problem-solvers.

Abstract: This paper introduces rStar, a self-play mutual reasoning approach that significantly improves reasoning capabilities of small language models (SLMs) without fine-tuning or superior models. rStar decouples reasoning into a self-play mutual generation-discrimination process. First, a target SLM augments the Monte Carlo Tree Search (MCTS) with a rich set of human-like reasoning actions to construct higher quality reasoning trajectories. Next, another SLM, with capabilities similar to the target SLM, acts as a discriminator to verify each trajectory generated by the target SLM. The mutually agreed reasoning trajectories are considered mutual consistent, thus are more likely to be correct. Extensive experiments across five SLMs demonstrate rStar can effectively solve diverse reasoning problems, including GSM8K, GSM-Hard, MATH, SVAMP, and StrategyQA. Remarkably, rStar boosts GSM8K accuracy from 12.51% to 63.91% for LLaMA2-7B, from 36.46% to 81.88% for Mistral-7B, from 74.53% to 91.13% for LLaMA3-8B-Instruct. Code will be available at this https URL .
Subjects: Computation and Language (cs.CL)
Cite as: [cs.CL]
  (or [cs.CL] for this version)
  Focus to learn more arXiv-issued DOI via DataCite

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • HTML (experimental)
  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

BibTeX formatted citation

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) The development of research self-efficacy scale

    self efficacy research paper pdf

  2. (PDF) Self-Efficacy In Online Learning Environments: A Literature Review

    self efficacy research paper pdf

  3. (PDF) Self-Efficacy and Performance of Research Skills among First

    self efficacy research paper pdf

  4. Self Efficacy and Why Believing in Yourself Matters Free Essay Example

    self efficacy research paper pdf

  5. (PDF) The Impact of Self-efficacy

    self efficacy research paper pdf

  6. (PDF) ENHANCING TEACHERS SELF-EFFICACY : THEORETICAL AND RESEARCH

    self efficacy research paper pdf

COMMENTS

  1. Self-efficacy and human motivation

    Learn how self-efficacy influences human motivation and performance from this comprehensive review of theory and research.

  2. (PDF) Self-efficacy

    PDF | Albert Bandura defined self-efficacy as a person's belief in his or her capability to successfully perform a particular task. Together with the... | Find, read and cite all the research you ...

  3. Self-Efficacy: The Power of Believing You Can

    PDF | The basic premise of self-efficacy theory is that "people's beliefs in their capabilities to produce desired effects by their own actions"... | Find, read and cite all the research you need ...

  4. The Confounded Self-Efficacy Construct: Review, Conceptual Analysis

    The purpose of this paper is to evaluate the validity of the self-efficacy-as-motivation argument in the context of health behaviour research. First, we discuss in more depth the conceptual definition of self-efficacy, its theorized independence and relationship with outcome expectancy, and its relevance to health behaviour.

  5. [PDF] Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change

    An integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment is presented and findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive mode of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. The present article presents an integrative theoretical ...

  6. PDF Self-Efficacy for Research: Development and Validation of a

    With that respect, in order to validate the developed research self-efficacy scale and investigate whether it measures researchers' self-efficacy as expected, the research self-efficacy of researchers with and without a Ph.D. degree were compared by using the second data set.

  7. PDF Self-efficacy: a Key Concept for Human Accomplishment and Success

    Impression management: This concept refers to the different ways individuals with low self-efficacy try to control how other people might perceive them. For example, an athlete with low self-efficacy may try to hide the fact that he/she has made a mistake rather than learn from the experience (receiving feedback from peers and coaches and engagement in critical self-reflection), which ...

  8. PDF Improving Self-Efficacy and Motivation

    Low self-efficacy beliefs, unfortunately, impede aca-demic achievement and, in the long run, create self-fulfilling prophecies of failure and learned helplessness that can devastate psychological well-being.

  9. Self-Efficacy, Satisfaction, and Academic Achievement: The Mediator

    Although there is considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, very few studies have explored the motivational mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy-achievement relationship, and they are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement.

  10. The General Academic Self-Efficacy Scale: Psychometric Properties

    Abstract Academic self-efficacy (ASE) refers to a student's global belief in his/her ability to master the various academic challenges at university and is an essential antecedent of wellbeing and performance. The five-item General Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (GASE) showed promise as a short and concise measure for overall ASE.

  11. The Impact of Self-efficacy

    PDF | Based on studies, the degree of self-efficacy appears to have a strong relationship with positive indicators of employees, such as their... | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ...

  12. [PDF] Self-Efficacy and Academic Motivation

    This article discusses academic motivation in terms of one type of personal expectancy: self-efficacy, defined as "People's judgments of their capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances" and discusses some efficacy research relevant to academic motivation. Expand.

  13. Self-Efficacy and Workplace Well-Being: Understanding the Role of

    Drawing from the emerging positive organizational behavior studies, the present research was designed to analyze the relationship between self-efficacy and resilience to promote workplace well-being. The main aim of the study was to examine the moderating role of resilience between self-efficacy and workplace well-being.

  14. Full article: The self-efficacy and academic performance reciprocal

    According to Bandura's theory of reciprocal determinism, self-efficacy and academic achievement can have a mutual influence over one another. While empirical research generally supports this positi...

  15. Self-Efficacy Theory

    Self-efficacy, 1. a concept derived from Social Learning Theory, provides a theoretical framework for behavior change. This framework can be used by the occupational health nurse to develop programs designed to effect behavior change. Research 2. studies focusing on behavior change (e.g., smoking cessation, weight control, exercise and cardiac ...

  16. PDF Challenges and Self-efficacy of Senior High School Students in Lcc

    Self-Efficacy Theory by Albert Bandura Albert Bandura has been a pioneer in self-efficacy research and Bandura has n their capabili motivation, cognitive resources, and agency to exert control over a given event. It is the belief in outcome or goal that i

  17. PDF Microsoft Word

    Self-efficacy theory postulates that people acquire information to evaluate efficacy. beliefs from four primary sources: (a) enactive mastery experiences (actual performances); (b) observation of others (vicarious experiences); (c) forms of persuasion, both verbal and otherwise;

  18. Self‐Efficacy: A Concept Analysis

    PDF | TOPIC. Concept analysis and self-efficacy. PURPOSE. This paper provides an in-depth analysis of the concept self-efficacy through the use of... | Find, read and cite all the research you ...

  19. PDF Evaluating Research Self-Efficacy in Undergraduate Students: Experience

    Students reporting research experiences were confident in their abilities to conduct research overall, and students with two or more experiences reported significantly higher levels of interest in research and research self-efficacy than students who had less or no experience. Qualitative analyses elucidated reasons for the observed differences.

  20. (PDF) Self-Efficacy Theory

    Self-efficacy belief provides the foundation for human - motivation, well-being and personal accomplishment. The theory explained, learning occurs in a social context with a dynamic and reciprocal interaction of the person, environment, and behaviour. It emphasizes on how these factors interact to determine motivation and behaviour.

  21. Self-Efficacy, Satisfaction, and Academic Achievement: The Mediator

    Abstract Although there is considerable evidence to support the direct effects of self-efficacy beliefs on academic achievement, very few studies have explored the motivational mechanism that mediates the self-efficacy-achievement relationship, and they are necessary to understand how and why self-efficacy affects students' academic achievement.

  22. (PDF) Self-Efficacy

    PDF | Purposive actions as well as positive self-esteem presuppose corresponding self-efficacy beliefs, that is, beliefs that one has the capacity to... | Find, read and cite all the research you ...

  23. Examining research motivation, self-efficacy, and anxiety in TEFL

    In our study, however, the relationship between research self-efficacy and motivation is positive. These evidence-based findings can help policymakers shape an evidence-based research policy. Recommendations and implications of the results for policymakers, research methodologists, research instructors, and MA students are discussed.

  24. (PDF) The Influence of Academic Self-Efficacy on Academic Performance

    This review integrates 12 years of research on the relationship between academic self-efficacy and university student's academic performance, and known cognitive and motivational variables that ...

  25. Mutual Reasoning Makes Smaller LLMs Stronger Problem-Solvers

    View PDF HTML (experimental) Abstract: This paper introduces rStar, a self-play mutual reasoning approach that significantly improves reasoning capabilities of small language models (SLMs) without fine-tuning or superior models. rStar decouples reasoning into a self-play mutual generation-discrimination process. First, a target SLM augments the ...

  26. Factors affecting teachers' self-efficacy in teaching in primary

    The quality of teaching is significantly influenced by teachers' self-efficacy. Grounded in social cognitive theory, this study examined the relationship between teachers' self-efficacy for ...