• Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

What Is Memory Consolidation?

Connecting new information to long-term memories

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Sean is a fact-checker and researcher with experience in sociology, field research, and data analytics.

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Tara Moore / Getty Images

Building Long-Term Memories With New Information

How memory consolidation works, influences on memory consolidation, speeding up memory consolidation.

Short-term memory tends to be limited in duration and capacity. In fact, the human brain can store short-term memories for only about 30 seconds—so it must move important information into  long-term memory to retain it. That's where memory consolidation comes in.

Memory consolidation is the process in the brain that converts short-term memories into long-term ones.

Memory consolidation relies on synapses in the brain. In something akin to an electrical system conducting a current,r the synapses pass signals from neuron to neuron with the help of neurotransmitters. 

The more frequently signals are passed, the stronger the synapses become. This process, called potentiation, is believed to play a major role in learning and memory processes.

When two neurons fire at the same time repeatedly, they become more likely to fire together in the future. Eventually, these two neurons become sensitized to one another.

As you acquire new experiences and memories, your brain creates more and more of these connections. Essentially, the brain rearranges itself, establishing new connections while weeding out old ones.

By rehearsing or recalling information repeatedly, these neural networks strengthen. For example, if you study the same material regularly over a long period, the pathways involved in remembering that information become stronger. The repeated firing of the same neurons makes it more likely that those same neurons will be able to repeat that firing in the future. As a result, you'll remember the information later with ease and accuracy.

Here's another way to think of these synaptic pathways: They're similar to a path in the woods. The more often you walk the path, the more familiar it becomes and the easier it is to traverse .

Although we often think of the brain as a filing cabinet or a computer with individual files of memories carefully stored away, the reality is that memories are spread out across the entire brain.

Through the consolidation process, the brain creates a sort of neural map, allowing memories to be retrieved when needed.

Sleep can play an important role in the consolidation process. One of the major theories of sleep suggests that it's a way to process and consolidate information we acquire during our waking lives.

People often think of memories as permanent, but just because a memory has been consolidated does not mean you can't lose it. Researchers have found that memories often need to be reconsolidated once they have been recalled. The process of recalling and reconsolidating a memory can help maintain and strengthen information in long-term memory.

Researchers have also found that memories must be reconsolidated every time you access them. This process, however, can transform and change the memory itself. The very act of remembering, it seems, can lead to some things being forgotten.

It is also possible to speed up the consolidation process when learning new information. Rehearsal and memorization strategies, such as using mnemonic devices and studying are a few techniques. One of the best ways to ensure that information is consolidated into long-term memory is to rehearse it over several spaced intervals repeatedly.

That's why going over your class notes once a week for several weeks will lead to greater memory retention than cramming the night before an exam.

A Word From Verywell

By understanding how consolidation works, you might be able to adapt your memorization strategies to boost the process. Repetition/study and sleep are good places to start.

Himmer, L., Schönauer, M., Heib, D. P. J., Schabus, M., & Gais, S. (2019). Rehearsal initiates systems memory consolidation, sleep makes it last .  Science Advances ,  5 (4), eaav1695. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aav1695

Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory . Physiol Rev . 2013;93(2):681-766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

Alberini CM, Ledoux JE. Memory reconsolidation . Curr Biol . 2013;23(17):R746-50. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.046

Milner B, Corkin S, Teuber HL. Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M . Neuropsychologia. 1968;6(3):215. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3

Payne J.D, Kensinger EA. Sleep’s role in the consolidation of emotional episodic memories . Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010;19(5):290-295. doi:10.1177/0963721410383978 

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • My Bibliography
  • Collections
  • Citation manager

Save citation to file

Email citation, add to collections.

  • Create a new collection
  • Add to an existing collection

Add to My Bibliography

Your saved search, create a file for external citation management software, your rss feed.

  • Search in PubMed
  • Search in NLM Catalog
  • Add to Search

Memory--a century of consolidation

Affiliation.

  • 1 the Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory and the Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA. [email protected]
  • PMID: 10634773
  • DOI: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248

The memory consolidation hypothesis proposed 100 years ago by Müller and Pilzecker continues to guide memory research. The hypothesis that new memories consolidate slowly over time has stimulated studies revealing the hormonal and neural influences regulating memory consolidation, as well as molecular and cellular mechanisms. This review examines the progress made over the century in understanding the time-dependent processes that create our lasting memories.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

  • Intense emotional experiences and enhanced training prevent memory loss induced by post-training amnesic treatments administered to the striatum, amygdala, hippocampus or substantia nigra. Prada-Alcala RA, Medina AC, Lopez NS, Quirarte GL. Prada-Alcala RA, et al. Rev Neurosci. 2012;23(5-6):501-8. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0061. Rev Neurosci. 2012. PMID: 23023883 Review.
  • Competitive interactions between endogenous LTD and LTP in the hippocampus underlie the storage of emotional memories and stress-induced amnesia. Diamond DM, Park CR, Campbell AM, Woodson JC. Diamond DM, et al. Hippocampus. 2005;15(8):1006-25. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20107. Hippocampus. 2005. PMID: 16086429 Review.
  • Stress generates emotional memories and retrograde amnesia by inducing an endogenous form of hippocampal LTP. Diamond DM, Park CR, Woodson JC. Diamond DM, et al. Hippocampus. 2004;14(3):281-91. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10186. Hippocampus. 2004. PMID: 15132427 Review.
  • Memory consolidation and the amygdala: a systems perspective. McGaugh JL. McGaugh JL. Trends Neurosci. 2002 Sep;25(9):456. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02211-7. Trends Neurosci. 2002. PMID: 12183206 Review.
  • Mechanisms of memory. Squire LR. Squire LR. Science. 1986 Jun 27;232(4758):1612-9. doi: 10.1126/science.3086978. Science. 1986. PMID: 3086978
  • How emotional contexts modulate item memory in individuals with high and low negative affect and worry. Becker N, Kühn S, Olsson A. Becker N, et al. Learn Mem. 2024 May 13;31(4):a053891. doi: 10.1101/lm.053891.123. Print 2024 Apr. Learn Mem. 2024. PMID: 38740426
  • Memory from nonsense syllables to novels: A survey of retention. Radvansky GA, Parra D, Doolen AC. Radvansky GA, et al. Psychon Bull Rev. 2024 May 7. doi: 10.3758/s13423-024-02514-3. Online ahead of print. Psychon Bull Rev. 2024. PMID: 38714636 Review.
  • Fronto-Cerebellar Diaschisis and Cognitive Dysfunction after Pontine Stroke: A Case Series and Systematic Review. Shimmyo K, Obayashi S. Shimmyo K, et al. Biomedicines. 2024 Mar 11;12(3):623. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines12030623. Biomedicines. 2024. PMID: 38540236 Free PMC article. Review.
  • Active forgetting and neuropsychiatric diseases. Berry JA, Guhle DC, Davis RL. Berry JA, et al. Mol Psychiatry. 2024 Mar 26. doi: 10.1038/s41380-024-02521-9. Online ahead of print. Mol Psychiatry. 2024. PMID: 38532011 Review.
  • Tunable Neuromorphic Switching Dynamics via Porosity Control in Mesoporous Silica Diffusive Memristors. Zhang T, Shao L, Jaafar A, Zeimpekis I, de Groot CH, Bartlett PN, Hector AL, Huang R. Zhang T, et al. ACS Appl Mater Interfaces. 2024 Apr 3;16(13):16641-16652. doi: 10.1021/acsami.3c19020. Epub 2024 Mar 17. ACS Appl Mater Interfaces. 2024. PMID: 38494599 Free PMC article.

Publication types

  • Search in MeSH

Related information

  • Cited in Books

Grants and funding

  • MH12526/MH/NIMH NIH HHS/United States

LinkOut - more resources

Full text sources.

  • Ovid Technologies, Inc.

Other Literature Sources

  • The Lens - Patent Citations
  • MedlinePlus Health Information
  • Citation Manager

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSH PMC Bookshelf Disclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.

Skip to main page content

100 Years of Consolidation— Remembering Müller and Pilzecker

  • Hilde A. Lechner 1 ,
  • Larry R. Squire 2 , and
  • John H. Byrne
  • 1 W.M. Keck Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University of Texas–Houston Medical School, Houston, Texas 77030 USA, 2 Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Diego, and Departments of Psychiatry, Neurosciences, and Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 USA

The origin of the concept of memory consolidation and the introduction of the term “consolidirung” (consolidation) to the modern science of memory are generally credited to Georg Elias Müller (1850–1934), professor at the University of Göttingen, Germany, and his student Alfons Pilzecker. Their seminal monograph “Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis” (Experimental Contributions to the Science of Memory), published in 1900, proposed that learning does not induce instantaneous, permanent memories but that memory takes time to be fixed (or consolidated). Consequently, memory remains vulnerable to disruption for a period of time after learning.

Georg Müller was among the founders of experimental psychology. Inspired by the work of Gustav Fechner (1801–1887), Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920), and Herman Ebbinghaus (1859–1909), 5 he was an early advocate of the view that mental function results from the action of matter, and that learning and memory should thus exhibit lawful properties. In their 300-page monograph, Müller and Pilzecker reported 40 experiments, carried out between 1892 and 1900, which were designed to identify the laws that govern memory formation and retrieval. Although Müller and Pilzecker have been frequently acknowledged for introducing the concept of memory consolidation, 2 their monograph has not been translated and knowledge of their methods and experimental findings has faded with the passing of almost a century. This commentary is intended to outline the experimental work of Müller and Pilzecker to explicate how the concept of memory consolidation originated. 3

In their studies, Müller and Pilzecker used lists of nonsense syllables, which had been introduced by Ebbinghaus (1885) . In his experiments, Ebbinghaus determined the number of trials that were needed to reproduce all of the syllables in a studied list twice without error (method of complete mastery). The methods of Müller and Pilzecker differed in three ways. First, they trained their subjects using nonsense syllables that were presented as paired associates. Lists were read aloud with emphasis on every odd-numbered syllable, thus creating pairs of emphasized and nonemphasized syllables, or trochees. To test memory, volunteers were cued with the first syllable of each pair, in a mixed order, and asked to recall the second syllable of the pair. Thus, if the list A-b-C-d-E-f were used for training (where each uppercase letter denotes the emphasized syllable of a pair), the correct answers to the cue syllables E, A, C would be the syllables f, b, and d. This novel procedure had originally been developed in Müller’s laboratory by Jost (1897) . Second, instead of counting the number of trials necessary to reproduce the paired associates correctly, Müller and Pilzecker fixed the number of training trials and quantified memory by determining the percentage of correctly recalled syllables, the percentage of incorrect answers, and the percentage of recall failures. This method is in such common use today that it is not often appreciated as an experimental innovation. Third, they constructed a sophisticated apparatus for determining the response latency for recall (see for details).

  • The Beginnings of Consolidation Theory

The line of evidence that would eventually result in the concept of memory consolidation began with a simple observation. Volunteers for the experiments of Müller and Pilzecker occasionally reported a strong tendency for syllable pairs to come to mind repeatedly between training sessions, despite efforts to suppress this intrusion. Müller and Pilzecker adopted the German word “perseveration” (p. 60) from descriptions of psychiatric illness to refer to this benign phenomenon. Pursuing the reports of their volunteers, they found evidence for perseveration during recall testing itself. For example, some individuals, recalled a syllable correctly in response to its proper cue but, in subsequent tests, continued to use the same syllable incorrectly in response to another cue (experiment 26). Also, incorrectly recalled syllables often belonged to the list that contained the correct syllable rather than being simply unrelated to previously trained material. This effect was strongest immediately after training, decayed during the subsequent minutes, and was not present after 1 day (Fig. 1 ; experiments 5, 7, 15, and 29). Müller and Pilzecker concluded that these peculiar mistakes resulted from the perseveration of recently learned material. Moreover, they speculated that perseveration was the result of transient activity in the brain that encoded the associative memory and suggested that this activity functioned as a short-term form of memory, similar to the primary memory proposed by William James (1842–1910). The sequence of our thoughts … is often interrupted by external or internal disruptions. Were it not for a spontaneous tendency of these thoughts to regain conscious awareness after a perturbation, such disturbances would prevent the completion of important trains of thought. (p. 75)

  • In this window
  • In a new window
  • Download as PowerPoint Slide

Evidence for perseveration. Subjects were tested for retention of paired syllables at various intervals after learning by cueing them with the first syllable of each pair. Incorrect answers were divided into syllables that belonged to the same list as the cue syllable (list correct errors) and syllables that did not belong to the studied list. Plotted here is a subset of list correct errors, in which the incorrectly recalled syllable belonged to the cue of the immediately preceding pair in the study list. The percentage of these errors decreased rapidly for ∼10 min following learning. This observation was interpreted as evidence for the perseveration of the studied syllables for ∼10 min. (From data reported on p. 65; experiments 5, 7, 15, and 29).

Importantly, Müller and Pilzecker suggested that the perseveration of syllable pairs might be necessary not only for establishing representations of the syllables in memory but for strengthening the associations between them. If so, they argued, disrupting perseveration should interfere with the formation of associative learning. The experience of everyday life shows that perseverative tendencies of different parts of a train of thought can be weakened considerably by turning one’s attention with energy to a different matter. … One can question, however, whether the effect of such an intense mental occupation … , immediately following reading of a list, simply reduces the frequency with which syllables of this list spontaneously regain conscious awareness. One might deem that the perseverative tendencies of syllables of a previously read list might also serve to consolidate the associations between these syllables … and that accordingly, weakening of the perseverative tendencies of syllables from a previously read list, due to other intense mental occupations, might have the additional effect of obstructing the associations between these syllables. (p. 68)

  • The Discovery of Retroactive Inhibition

Müller and Pilzecker designed 10 experiments to test the hypothesis that perseveration reflected an internal, physiological process that serves to strengthen associative memories. Three representative experiments are described here. In experiment 31 (Fig. 2 ), a volunteer received eight training trials with six pairs of syllables (list A), which took ∼1 min. After an interval of 34 sec a second list (list X) was read eight times to prevent the perseveration of syllables from list A. Then, after a longer interval of 6 min, cue syllables from both lists A and X were presented in mixed order, and the percentage of correctly recalled syllables from list A was determined. To examine the effect of the interpolation of list X on recall of list A, a third list (list B) was trained eight times and recall was tested after an equivalent interval, but without interpolated learning. Retention of associations from list A was lower than retention from list B (23% vs. 48%), suggesting that the interpolation of list X had interfered with memory for the associations from list A. Similar results were obtained when the interval between list A and X was 1 min, and the retention interval was 24 hr. Müller and Pilzecker commented: The suspicion imposed on us by these results was that the processes that serve the formation of associations in a list of syllables continue for some time after reading of the list, but that they would be weakened by another intense mental occupation, such that an intense mental occupation following reading of a list of syllables would inhibit associations of that list. Because this type of inhibition acts on the effect of an ostensibly completed process … we will call it … “retroactive inhibition.” (p. 179)

Retroactive inhibition of verbal material. Lists of 12 syllables were mounted on rotating drums and read aloud as pairs by emphasizing every other syllable. A subject was given eight training trials with syllable pairs from list A (total training time, ∼1 min) followed by eight training trials with an interpolated list X. Retention was then tested 6 min after studying list X and compared to retention of a third list B, which was tested without an interfering list. Retention of list A was poorer than retention of list B, suggesting a retroactive inhibition of list X on the associations from list A. (From data reported on p. 181; experiment 31).

In another study (experiment 34), Müller and Pilzecker determined that retention (measured at 1.5 hr) of the first of two lists was impaired if the second list followed after a brief interval of 17 sec but was not affected when the second list followed after a longer interval of 6 min. This experiment suggested … that the associations of a [list] of syllables are being inhibited less by a subsequently read list, the later the [interpolated list] is being read. [This is] because the longer the interval between the first and second list, the more those processes that induce and strengthen the associations of the first list will have subsided at the time when the second list is being read and accordingly, the lesser … will be the disrupting effect of reading the second list upon these processes. (p. 184)

  • Retroactive Inhibition by Unrelated Material

To complete their examination of retroactive inhibition, Müller and Pilzecker asked whether the material used to suppress perseveration had to be similar to the learned material (experiment 35). To answer this question, list A was read eight times and immediately followed by the sequential presentation of three pictures showing landscapes (Fig. 3 ). Subjects were asked to describe each picture for a total of 6 min, after which retention for list A was tested. A control list (list B) was tested for retention after 6 min without intermittent distraction. Again, retention of list A was lower than retention of list B (24% vs. 56%), suggesting that mental activity unrelated to the learned material itself could be disruptive.

Retroactive inhibition by pictorial material. Syllables from list A were trained and followed immediately by a 6-min presentation of three painted landscapes. The subject was asked to describe the pictures. Retention of list A was then compared to retention of a control list B. The result suggested that even material not related to what had been studied could interfere with retention. (From data reported on p. 188; experiment 35).

Summarizing these results, Müller and Pilzecker reached the following conclusion: After all this, there is no alternative but to assume that after reading a list of syllables certain physiological processes, which serve to strengthen the associations induced during reading of that list, continue with decreasing intensity for a period of time. These processes and their facilitating effects on these associations are being weakened to a greater or lesser extent if the experimental subject experiences further mental exertion immediately after reading a list. …

It seems justified to suppose that the physiological processes mentioned here are the same that underlie perseverative tendencies. . . . . Mental exertion in an experimental subject after reading a list of syllables has firstly the direct effect of weakening the perseverative tendencies of these syllables and secondly, because the effect of these perseverative tendencies is to consolidate syllable associations, the additional effect of impairing these associations. It is the same, quickly fading perseverative tendency that is responsible for the fact that, within about 10 minutes after reading a list of syllables, the recall latency is shorter the earlier testing is done; and the impairment of [the perseverative tendency] … handicaps the associations of a list of syllables within the first 10 minutes of reading the list (pp. 196–197).

This conclusion implies that memory is consolidated with a time course of ∼10 min. Yet it should be noted that the longest interval between original learning and interpolated activity that Müller and Pilzecker studied was 6 min and that retroactive inhibition was not detected at intervals longer than 1 min. Müller and Pilzecker’s value of 10 min seems to have been an estimate drawn from the observed time course of perseveration (see Fig. 1 ).

McDougall (1901) and Burnham (1903) immediately recognized that perseveration theory provided a way to understand the temporally graded retrograde amnesia (i.e., the loss of premorbid memory) that results from traumatic head injury. Systematic laboratory studies of retrograde amnesia using electroconvulsive shock (ECS) in rats were pioneered by Carl Duncan and generally supported the estimates of Müller and Pilzecker for the time course of consolidation. In these experiments, temporally graded retrograde amnesia for an active avoidance task was produced by ECS up to 15 min following learning but not at intervals longer than 1 hr ( Duncan 1949 ). Duncan’s experiments ushered in decades of work in many laboratories to define the time course of memory consolidation by studying ECS-induced retrograde amnesia. The data from these experiments, however, did not converge on a single time course that might have helped to identify the mechanisms underlying memory consolidation. Instead, the time course of memory consolidation appeared to vary widely depending on task parameters (for reviews, see Chorover 1976 ; McGaugh and Gold 1976 ).

Although the concepts of perseveration and memory consolidation remained central to the study of learning and memory (for reviews, see Glickman 1961 ; McGaugh and Herz 1972 ; Weingartner and Parker 1984 ), they would eventually be replaced by interference theory as an explanation for retroactive inhibition. Interference theory proposed that memories acquired close together in time compete for representational space, thereby interfering with each other. This theory readily accounted for the original observation of Müller and Pilzecker of a time-dependent disruption of original learning by interpolated material, and could also account for later findings, such as the fact that the degree of disruption increases with greater similarity between interpolated and original material (for reviews, see McGeoch and Irion 1952 ; Keppel 1984 ). 4 Thus, the perseveration theory of Müller and Pilzecker of memory consolidation fell out of favor within experimental psychology; however, their concepts eventually found considerable utility in biologically oriented studies of memory.

In 1949, in the light of emerging physiological data, Donald Hebb (1904–1985) advanced the dual-trace theory of memory formation. This theory revived the idea of perseveration by postulating a short-term memory in the form of reverberatory activity within local neural circuits. Reverberation was proposed to induce structural changes at synapses in the reverberating network and thus permit the memory to be stored more permanently ( Hebb 1949 ). Hebb’s ideas marked a shift in the approach to memory consolidation from manipulating memory in intact animals to studying the biological events that underlie the phenomenon of consolidation. For example, the postulate of structural change suggested a role for protein synthesis in the fixing of memory, which was confirmed by experiment ( Flexner et al. 1963 ; Agranoff and Klinger 1964 ; Barondes and Cohen 1965 ; for review, see Davis and Squire 1984 ). Today, it is thought that the phenomenon of memory consolidation in the minutes and hours after learning depends, in part, on a cascade of molecular events that lead to changes in the properties of neurons (e.g., Bailey and Chen 1983 ; Goelet et al. 1986 ; Alberini et al. 1994 ; O’Leary et al. 1995 ; Tully et al. 1995 ; Dudai 1996 ). These events include activation by second messenger cascades of transcriptional regulators, and multiple waves of mRNA and protein synthesis. Some of the proteins produce functional changes in neurons and synapses, whereas others lead to structural changes. In this sense, the ideas of Müller and Pilzecker can be traced from their origin to contemporary studies of the neuronal events that occur during a brief time period, shortly after learning.

Yet the term consolidation is also used today in the context of observations that memory can remain vulnerable to some manipulations for weeks, months, and even years after learning. In 1957, Scoville and Milner described severe anterograde and temporally limited retrograde amnesia following bilateral removal of medial temporal lobe structures. This finding led ultimately to the identification of the hippocampus and adjacent anatomically related cortical structures of the medial temporal lobe as components of a memory system that is essential for the formation of long-term memory ( Squire and Zola-Morgan 1991 ). Disrupting the function of this system causes retrograde amnesia, which is most severe for recent events and gradually less severe for remote events. Graded retrograde amnesia can extend across several years in humans ( Russel and Nathan 1946 ; Squire et al. 1975 ) and across weeks in mice ( Squire and Spanis 1984 ; for review, see Milner et al. 1998 ). Together, these findings suggest that medial temporal lobe structures contribute to the prolonged and gradual consolidation of memory over the course of weeks or even years. Specifically, the hippocampus has been proposed to direct a gradual process of reorganization and stabilization of representations in the neocortex (for reviews, see Squire and Alvarez 1995 ; Knowlton and Fanselow 1998 ). Although this process is likely to involve changes at synapses, it depends fundamentally on the successful interaction between the hippocampus and the neocortex and is distinct from the process by which synaptic efficacy is altered during the early phases of memory formation.

Müller and Pilzecker’s perseveration theory of memory consolidation was the starting point for discoveries of molecular and cellular events and brain systems important for memory formation. The neuronal events and processes that occur at the level of brain systems may unfold across different time scales, but all contribute to the dynamic and selective process of fixing memory over time. A challenge remaining for the next century is to understand, in detail, all the processes that underlie the phenomenon of memory consolidation—molecular and cellular events at synapses, the operations of brain systems, and the interactions among them.

  • Acknowledgments

We thank E.R. Kandel for his comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants NS19895 (J.H.B.), MH58321 (J.H.B.), NIH grant 24600 (L.R.S.), and the Medical Research Service of the Department of Veterans Affairs (L.R.S.).

↵ 1 Corresponding author.

↵ 2 The Science Citation Index lists 46 citations since 1955.

↵ 5 For biographical sketches and bibliographies, see Boring (1950) .

↵ 3 Earlier discussions of Müller and Pilzecker can also be found in McDougall (1901) ; Glickman (1961) ; and McGaugh and Herz (1972) .

↵ 4 Müller and Pilzecker themselves proposed an interference theory based on several experiments in which a cue syllable was trained with two different associates. These experiments showed that two associations with the same cue sylable were less well remembered than a single association. Müller and Pilzecker interpreted these findings as evidence for a competitive interference between the alternative syllables during acquisition and/or retrieval (see ).

  • Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press
  • Agranoff B.W. ,
  • Klinger P.D.
  • Alberini C.M. ,
  • Ghirardi M. ,
  • Kandel E.R.
  • Bailey C.H. ,
  • Barondes S.H. ,
  • Boring E.G.
  • Burnham W.H.
  • Chorover S.
  • Rosenzweig M.L. ,
  • Bennett E.L.
  • Davis H.P. ,
  • Squire L.R.
  • Duncan C.P.
  • Ebbinghaus H.
  • Flexner J.B. ,
  • Flexner L.B. ,
  • Glickman S.E.
  • Goelet P. ,
  • Castellucci V.F. ,
  • Schacher S. ,
  • Weingartner H. ,
  • Parker E.S.
  • Knowlton B.J. ,
  • Fanselow M.S.
  • McDougall W.
  • McGaugh J.L. ,
  • McGeoch J.A. ,
  • Milner B. ,
  • Squire L.R. ,
  • Müller G.E. ,
  • Pilzecker A.
  • O’Leary F.A. ,
  • Byrne J.H. ,
  • Cleary L.J.
  • Russel W.R. ,
  • Nathan P.W.
  • Scoville W.B. ,
  • Spanis C.W.
  • Zola-Morgan S.
  • Slater P.C. ,
  • Boynton S.C. ,
  • Del Vecchio M.

What's this?

This Article

  • doi: 10.1101/lm.6.2.77 Learn. Mem. 1999. 6: 77-87 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press
  • Extract Free
  • » Full Text Free
  • Full Text (PDF) Free

Article Category

  • PERSPECTIVE
  • Alert me when this article is cited
  • Alert me if a correction is posted
  • Similar articles in this journal
  • Similar articles in Web of Science
  • Similar articles in PubMed
  • Download to citation manager
  • Permissions

Citing Articles

  • Load citing article information
  • Citing articles via Web of Science
  • Citing articles via Google Scholar

Google Scholar

  • Articles by Lechner, H. A.
  • Articles by Byrne, J. H.
  • Search for related content

PubMed/NCBI

  • PubMed citation

Navigate This Article

Current issue.

  • June 2024, 31 (6)
  • Alert me to new issues of Learning & Memory
  • Author Information
  • Subscriptions
  • E-mail Alerts & RSS Feeds

Sutter

Copyright © 2024 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

  • Print ISSN: 1072-0502
  • Online ISSN: 1549-5485

Psychology Dictionary

PERSEVERATION-CONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS

the hypothesis postulating that data goes through two phases in memory cultivation. During the first phase, the memory is retained by perseveration of neural activity and is simply disturbed. During the second phase, the memory comes to be fixed, or integrated, and is no longer simply disturbed. Commonly referred to as consolidation hypothesis- or consolidation-perseveration hypothesis.

Avatar photo

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Latest Posts

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

A Quick Look at the History Behind Hypnosis

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

A Brief History of Brainwashing: The Science of Thought Control

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

A Deep Dive into the Social Psychology of Leadership

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Counseling Approaches to Client Care: Theories to Apply in Practice

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

The Future Of Education: Can You Earn A Psychology Degree Online?

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Insomnia & Mental Illness: What is the Correlation?

Psychology of Decision Making

Stop Guessing: Here Are 3 Steps to Data-Driven Psychological Decisions

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Getting Help with Grief: Understanding Therapy & How It Can Help

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Exploring the Psychology of Risk and Reward

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Understanding ADHD in Women: Symptoms, Treatment & Support

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Meeting the Milestones: A Guide to Piaget's Child Developmental Stages

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Counseling, Therapy, and Psychology: What Is The Difference?

Popular psychology terms, medical model, hypermnesia, affirmation, brainwashing, backup reinforcer, message-learning approach, affiliative behavior, spontaneous neural activity, behavioral modeling, kinesthetic feedback, approach motivation.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: March 2009

A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation

  • Karim Nader 1 &
  • Oliver Hardt 1  

Nature Reviews Neuroscience volume  10 ,  pages 224–234 ( 2009 ) Cite this article

11k Accesses

569 Citations

47 Altmetric

Metrics details

The terms consolidation and reconsolidation refer to transient neurobiological processes that are thought to implement changes in synaptic efficacy in neurons that participate in forming a memory, thereby over time stabilizing the memory and rendering it relatively permanent (or long-term). Consolidation follows initial memory acquisition, whereas reconsolidation follows reactivation of a memory that already has been consolidated, as during memory recall or retrieval.

Although most researchers studying the neuroscience of memory have embraced the concept of memory consolidation, reconsolidation was initially met with strong skepticism. The idea that long-term memory, regardless of its remoteness, can enter states of plasticity after reactivation, similar to the states observed shortly after acquisition, challenges the consolidation model, which proposes an irreversible memory-stabilization process that fixes memory permanently.

The concept that both consolidation and reconsolidation exist as time-dependent stabilization processes is deduced from structurally equivalent data sets by applying identical operant definitions and basic assumptions. These data sets show that amnesic treatments can affect memory only when they are applied shortly after memory acquisition (consolidation) or reactivation (reconsolidation), and that there is intact short-term but impaired long-term memory following the amnesic treatment.

Alternative explanations for the memory impairment that follows post-reactivation amnesic treatment have been proposed (for example, that the cause is treatment-induced lesions, transient retrieval blockade, facilitated extinction or new learning), but none of these interpretations can explain all of the available reactivation-induced memory-instability data (unlike the reconsolidation hypothesis itself). Furthermore, as reconsolidation and consolidation are deduced from very similar data sets using identical operant definitions, these interpretations also fall short of fully explaining results from consolidation studies.

Some argue that the existence of boundary conditions that moderate the occurrence of reconsolidation processes limits the validity rather than informing about the nature of reconsolidation. Therefore the criticism — based on the terminology — that reconsolidation should exactly recapitulate consolidation and occur for each and every memory is not justified.

The existing consolidation framework cannot accommodate the reconsolidation data. A new neurobiological model of memory is needed that acknowledges the long-held notion prominent in cognitive psychology that memory, at any age, is in essence plastic.

Consolidated memories can re-enter states of transient instability following reactivation, from which they must again stabilize in order to persist, contradicting the previously dominant view that memory and its associated plasticity mechanisms progressively and irreversibly decline with time. We witness exciting times, as neuroscience begins embracing a position, long-held in cognitive psychology, that recognizes memory as a principally dynamic process. In light of remaining controversy, we here establish that the same operational definitions and types of evidence underpin the deduction of both reconsolidation and consolidation, thus validating the extrapolation that post-retrieval memory plasticity reflects processes akin to those that stabilized the memory following acquisition.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 print issues and online access

176,64 € per year

only 14,72 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on Springer Link
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Similar content being viewed by others

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Rapid neural reorganization during retrieval practice predicts subsequent long-term retention and false memory

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Engram neurons: Encoding, consolidation, retrieval, and forgetting of memory

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Developmental differences in memory reactivation relate to encoding and inference in the human brain

Duncan, C. P. The retroactive effect of electroconvulsive shock. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 42 , 32–44 (1949).

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

White, N. M. & McDonald, R. J. Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 77 , 125–184 (2002).

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Davis, M. Neurobiology of fear responses: the role of the amygdala. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9 , 382–402 (1997).

LeDoux, J. E. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23 , 155–184 (2000).

Maren, S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24 , 897–931 (2001).

Morris, R. G. Episodic-like memory in animals: psychological criteria, neural mechanisms and the value of episodic-like tasks to investigate animal models of neurodegenerative disease. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356 , 1453–1465 (2001).

CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Thompson, R. F. & Krupa, D. J. Organization of memory traces in the mammalian brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 17 , 519–549 (1994).

Ebbinghaus, M. Über das Gedächtnis (Buehler, Leipzig, 1885).

Google Scholar  

Glickman, S. Perseverative neural processes and consolidation of the memory trace. Psychol. Bull. 58 , 218–233 (1961).

McGaugh, J. L. Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science 153 , 1351–1358 (1966).

Pastalkova, E. et al. Storage of spatial information by the maintenance mechanism of LTP. Science 313 , 1141–1144 (2006). This paper identified PKMζ as the only molecule currently known to maintain LTM.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Scoville, W. B. & Milner, B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Psychiatry 20 , 11–21 (1957).

CAS   Google Scholar  

Squire, L. R. & Alvarez, P. Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5 , 169–177 (1995).

Dudai, Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55 , 51–86 (2004).

Kandel, E. R. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science 294 , 1030–1038 (2001).

Flexner, L. B., Flexner, J. B. & Stellar, E. Memory and cerebral protein synthesis in mice as affected by graded amounts of puromycin. Exp. Neurol. 13 , 264–272 (1965).

Gordon, W. C. & Spear, N. E. Effect of reactivation of a previously acquired memory on the interaction between memories in the rat. J. Exp. Psychol. 99 , 349–355 (1973).

McGaugh, J. L. & Krivanek, J. A. Strychnine effects on discrimination learning in mice: effects of dose and time of administration. Physiol. Behav. 5 , 1437–1442 (1970).

Spear, N. & Mueller, C. in Memory Consolidation: Psychobiology of Cognition (eds Weingarten, H. & Parker, E.) 111–147 (Laurence Erlbaum Associates, London, 1984).

Goelet, P., Castellucci, V. F., Schacher, S. & Kandel, E. R. The long and short of long-term memory- a molecular framework. Nature 322 , 419–422 (1986).

McGaugh, J. L. Memory - a century of consolidation. Science 287 , 248–251 (2000).

Dudai, Y. & Morris, R. in Brain, Perception, Memory: Advances in Cognitive Sciences (ed. Bolhius, J.) 149–162 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2000).

Davis, H. P. & Squire, L. R. Protein synthesis and memory. A review. Psychol. Bull. 96 , 518–559 (1984).

Klann, E. & Sweatt, J. D. Altered protein synthesis is a trigger for long-term memory formation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89 , 247–259 (2007).

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Guzowski, J. F. & McGaugh, J. L. Antisense oligodeoxynucleotide-mediated disruption of hippocampal cAMP response element binding protein levels impairs consolidation of memory for water maze training. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94 , 2693–2698 (1997).

Yin, J. C. P., Del Vecchio, M., Zhou, H. & Tully, T. CREB as a Memory Modulator: induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in Drosophilia . Cell 81 , 107–115 (1995).

Silva, A. J., Kogan, J. H., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. CREB and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21 , 127–148 (1998).

McGaugh, J. L. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27 , 1–28 (2004).

Martin, S. J., Grimwood, P. D. & Morris, R. G. Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23 , 649–711 (2000).

Malenka, R. C. & Nicoll, R. A. Long-term potentiation--a decade of progress? Science 285 , 1870–1874 (1999).

Milner, B., Squire, L. R. & Kandel, E. R. Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron 20 , 445–468 (1998).

Shors, T. J. & Matzel, L. D. Long-term potentiation: what's learning got to do with it? Behav. Brain Sci. 20 , 597–614; discussion 614–655 (1997).

Routtenberg, A. & Rekart, J. L. Post-translational protein modification as the substrate for long-lasting memory. Trends Neurosci. 28 , 12–19 (2005).

Schafe, G. E. & LeDoux, J. E. Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 20 , RC96 (2000).

Nader, K., Schafe, G. E. & Le Doux, J. E. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406 , 722–726 (2000). Sparking widespread renewed interest in reconsolidation, this seminal paper provided the first analytical demonstration of the phenomenon. It used localized infusions of anisomycin into the LBA, the site that putatively mediates memory consolidation for auditory fear conditioning.

Bozon, B., Davis, S. & Laroche, S. A requirement for the immediate early gene zif268 in reconsolidation of recognition memory after retrieval. Neuron 40 , 695–701 (2003).

Debiec, J., LeDoux, J. E. & Nader, K. Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron 36 , 527–538 (2002).

Kida, S. et al. CREB required for the stability of new and reactivated fear memories. Nature Neurosci. 5 , 348–355 (2002).

Lee, J. L., Everitt, B. J. & Thomas, K. L. Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Science 304 , 839–843 (2004). This paper showed that the mechanisms that mediate consolidation and reconsolidation can be doubly dissociated, suggesting that the differences between consolidation and reconsolidation cannot be explained by use of asymmetric protocols.

Sangha, S., Scheibenstock, A. & Lukowiak, K. Reconsolidation of a long-term memory in Lymnaea requires new protein and RNA synthesis and the soma of right pedal dorsal 1. J. Neurosci. 23 , 8034–8040 (2003).

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A. & Stickgold, R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425 , 616–620 (2003). This paper was the first to demonstrate reconsolidation in humans in a procedural memory task. It showed that learning of a new motor sequence after reactivation of an old one reduces memory accuracy for the reactivated old sequence on a later test, and that this effect is dependent on reactivation.

Child., F. M., Epstein, H. T., Kuzirian, A. M. & Alkon, D. L. Memory reconsolidation in Hermissenda . Biol. Bull. 205 , 218–219 (2003).

Bailey, C. H. & Chen, M. Morphological basis of long-term habituation and sensitization in Aplysia . Science 220 , 91–93 (1983).

Nader, K. Memory traces unbound. Trends Neurosci. 26 , 65–72 (2003).

Bailey, C. H. & Kandel, E. R. Structural changes accompanying memory storage. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 55 , 397–426 (1993).

Lee, S. H. et al. Synaptic protein degradation underlies destabilization of retrieved fear memory. Science 319 , 1253–1256 (2008). This paper showed that proteins must be degraded in order to transform a reactivated memory from a fixed to a labile state, suggesting that after reactivation degraded proteins need to be replaced through protein synthesis or else the reactivated memory cannot be re-stabilized.

Gordon, W. C. Susceptibility of a reactivated memory to the effects of strychnine: a time-dependent phenomenon. Physiol. Behav. 18 , 95–99 (1977).

Rodriguez, W. A., Horne, C. A. & Padilla, J. L. Effects of glucose and fructose on recently reactivated and recently acquired memories. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 23 , 1285–1317 (1999).

Horne, C. A., Rodriguez, W. A., Wright, T. P. & Padilla, J. L. Time-dependent effects of fructose on the modulation of a reactivated memory. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 21 , 649–658 (1997).

Rodriguez, W. A., Rodriguez, S. B., Phillips, M. Y. & Martinez, J. L. Jr. Post-reactivation cocaine administration facilitates later acquisition of an avoidance response in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 59 , 125–129 (1993).

Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R. & Lewis, D. J. Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160 , 203–204 (1968). This seminal paper provided the first description of the reconsolidation phenomenon but, for historical reasons, reconsolidation remained outside the mainstream literature until 2000.

Gordon, W. C. Similarities of recently acquired and reactivated memories in interference. Am. J. Psychol. 90 , 231–242 (1977).

Spear, N. Retrieval of memory in animals. Psychol. Rev. 80 , 163–194 (1973).

Lewis, D. J. Psychobiology of active and inactive memory. Psychol. Bull. 86 , 1054–1083 (1979). This conceptual paper was one of the earliest theoretical attempts to explain both the consolidation and the reconsolidation data sets.

Fonseca, R., Nagerl, U. V. & Bonhoeffer, T. Neuronal activity determines the protein synthesis dependence of long-term potentiation. Nature Neurosci. 9 , 478–480 (2006). An elegant LTP study which found that administration of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin after and only after reactivation of an already-potentiated pathway attenuated LTP, suggesting that reconsolidation effects are observed for LTP as well.

Doyere, V., Debiec, J., Monfils, M. H., Schafe, G. E. & LeDoux, J. E. Synapse-specific reconsolidation of distinct fear memories in the lateral amygdala. Nature Neurosci. 10 , 414–416 (2007). This paper was the first to demonstrate that blocking reconsolidation reverses learning-induced changes in field potentials.

Miller, C. A. & Marshall, J. F. Molecular substrates for retrieval and reconsolidation of cocaine-associated contextual memory. Neuron 47 , 873–884 (2005).

Valjent, E. et al. Plasticity-associated gene Krox24/Zif268 is required for long-lasting behavioral effects of cocaine. J. Neurosci. 26 , 4956–4960 (2006).

Rose, J. K. & Rankin, C. H. Blocking memory reconsolidation reverses memory-associated changes in glutamate receptor expression. J. Neurosci. 26 , 11582–11587 (2006). This paper showed that blocking reconsolidation in nematodes reverses the molecular correlates of LTM to those of naive animals.

Lewis, D. J., Bregman, N. J. & Mahan, J. J. Jr. Cue-dependent amnesia in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 2 , 243–247 (1972).

Dawson, R. G. & McGaugh, J. L. Electroconvulsive shock effects on a reactivated memory trace: further examination. Science 166 , 525–527 (1969).

Gold, P. E. & King, R. A. Amnesia: tests of the effect of delayed footshock-electroconvulsive shock pairings. Physiol. Behav. 8 , 797–800 (1972).

De Vietti, T. & Holiday, J. H. Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace: a replication. Psychon. Sci. 29 , 137–138 (1972).

Miller, R. R. & Springer, A. D. Amnesia, consolidation, and retrieval. Psychol. Rev. 80 , 69–79 (1973).

Eisenberg, M., Kobilo, T., Berman, D. E. & Dudai, Y. Stability of retrieved memory: inverse correlation with trace dominance. Science 301 , 1102–1104 (2003). This elegant study showed in two different species that consolidation of extinction learning for a memory could inhibit reconsolidation for this memory.

Pederia, M. E. & Maldonado, H. Protein synthesis subserves reconsolidation or extinction depending on reminder duration. Neuron 38 , 863–869 (2003).

Milekic, M. H. & Alberini, C. M. Temporally graded requirement for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron 36 , 521–525 (2002).

Suzuki, A. et al. Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. J. Neurosci. 24 , 4787–4795 (2004).

Stollhoff, N., Menzel, R. & Eisenhardt, D. Spontaneous recovery from extinction depends on the reconsolidation of the acquisition memory in an appetitive learning paradigm in the honeybee ( Apis mellifera ). J. Neurosci. 25 , 4485–4492 (2005).

Duvarci, S., Mamou, C. B. & Nader, K. Extinction is not a sufficient condition to prevent fear memories from undergoing reconsolidation in the basolateral amygdala. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24 , 249–260 (2006).

Debiec, J., Doyere, V., Nader, K. & Ledoux, J. E. Directly reactivated, but not indirectly reactivated, memories undergo reconsolidation in the amygdala. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103 , 3428–3433 (2006).

Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O. & Nadel, L. Reconsolidation of episodic memories: a subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learn. Mem. 14 , 47–53 (2007).

Forcato, C., Argibay, P. F., Pedreira, M. E. & Maldonado, H. Human reconsolidation does not always occur when a memory is retrieved: the relevance of the reminder structure. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91 , 50–57 (2008).

Pedreira, M. E., Perez-Cuesta, L. M. & Maldonado, H. Mismatch between what is expected and what actually occurs triggers memory reconsolidation or extinction. Learn. Mem. 11 , 579–585 (2004).

Morris, R. G. et al. Memory reconsolidation: sensitivity of spatial memory to inhibition of protein synthesis in dorsal hippocampus during encoding and retrieval. Neuron 50 , 479–489 (2006).

Hupbach, A., Hardt, O., Gomez, R. & Nadel, L. The dynamics of memory: context-dependent updating. Learn. Mem. 15 , 574–579 (2008). This paper demonstrated in humans that episodic memory reconsolidation depends on re-exposure to the spatial context in which the original learning occurred, suggesting that the spatial context is crucial for inducing reconsolidation in human episodic memory.

Lee, J. L., Di Ciano, P., Thomas, K. L. & Everitt, B. J. Disrupting reconsolidation of drug memories reduces cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuron 47 , 795–801 (2005).

Pedreira, M. E., Perez-Cuesta, L. M. & Maldonado, H. Reactivation and reconsolidation of long-term memory in the crab Chasmagnathus : protein synthesis requirement and mediation by NMDA-type glutamatergic receptors. J. Neurosci. 22 , 8305–8311 (2002).

Rudy, J. W., Biedenkapp, J. C., Moineau, J. & Bolding, K. Anisomycin and the reconsolidation hypothesis. Learn. Mem. 13 , 1–3 (2006).

Duvarci, S. & Nader, K. Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. J. Neurosci. 24 , 9269–9275 (2004).

Gordon, W. C. & Spear, N. E. The effects of strychnine on recently acquired and reactivated passive avoidance memories. Physiol. Behav. 10 , 1071–1075 (1973).

Power, A. E., Berlau, D. J., McGaugh, J. L. & Steward, O. Anisomycin infused into the hippocampus fails to block “reconsolidation” but impairs extinction: the role of re-exposure duration. Learn. Mem. 13 , 27–34 (2006).

Fischer, A., Sananbenesi, F., Schrick, C., Spiess, J. & Radulovic, J. Distinct roles of hippocampal de novo protein synthesis and actin rearrangement in extinction of contextual fear. J. Neurosci. 24 , 1962–1966 (2004).

Vianna, M. R., Szapiro, G., McGaugh, J. L., Medina, J. H. & Izquierdo, I. Retrieval of memory for fear-motivated training initiates extinction requiring protein synthesis in the rat hippocampus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98 , 12251–12254 (2001).

McGaugh, J. L. Memory reconsolidation hypothesis revived but restrained: theoretical comment on Biedenkapp and Rudy (2004). Behav. Neurosci. 118 , 1140–1142 (2004).

Prado-Alcala, R. A. et al. Amygdala or hippocampus inactivation after retrieval induces temporary memory deficit. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86 , 144–149 (2006).

Anokhin, K. V., Tiunova, A. A. & Rose, S. P. Reminder effects - reconsolidation or retrieval deficit? Pharmacological dissection with protein synthesis inhibitors following reminder for a passive-avoidance task in young chicks. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15 , 1759–1765 (2002).

Cahill, L., McGaugh, J. L. & Weinberger, N. M. The neurobiology of learning and memory: some reminders to remember. Trends Neurosci. 24 , 578–581 (2001).

Squire, L. R. Lost forever or temporarily misplaced? The long debate about the nature of memory impairment. Learn. Mem. 13 , 522–529 (2006).

Nader, K. & Wang, S. H. Fading in. Learn. Mem. 13 , 530–535 (2006).

Gold, P. & King, R. Storage failure versus retrieval failure. Psychol. Rev. 81 , 465–469 (1974).

Miller, R. & Springer, A. Implications of recovery from experimental amnesia. Psychol. Rev. 81 , 470–473 (1974).

de Hoz, L., Martin, S. J. & Morris, R. G. Forgetting, reminding, and remembering: the retrieval of lost spatial memory. PLoS Biol. 2 , 1233–1242 (2004).

Lattal, K. M. & Abel, T. Behavioral impairments caused by injections of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin after contextual retrieval reverse with time. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101 , 4667–4672 (2004).

Bailey, C. H., Bartsch, D. & Kandel, E. R. Toward a molecular definition of long-term memory storage. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93 , 13445–13452 (1996).

Fonseca, R., Nagerl, U. V., Morris, R. G. & Bonhoeffer, T. Competing for memory: hippocampal LTP under regimes of reduced protein synthesis. Neuron 44 , 1011–1020 (2004).

Riccio, D. C., Millin, P. M. & Bogart, A. R. Reconsolidation: a brief history, a retrieval view, and some recent issues. Learn. Mem. 13 , 536–544 (2006).

Tronson, N. C., Wiseman, S. L., Olausson, P. & Taylor, J. R. Bidirectional behavioral plasticity of memory reconsolidation depends on amygdalar protein kinase A. Nature Neurosci. 9 , 167–169 (2006). This paper provided the first demonstration that, like new memories, reactivated old memories could be enhanced by activating a kinase signalling pathway, showing that reactivation-induced plasticity allows memory modulation.

Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned Reflexes (Dover, New York, 1927).

Bouton, M. E. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol. Bull. 114 , 80–99 (1993).

Rescorla, R. A. in Contemporary Learning Theories (eds Mowrer, R. R. & Klein, S. B.) 119–155 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey, 2000).

Myers, K. M. & Davis, M. Systems-level reconsolidation: reengagement of the hippocampus with memory reactivation. Neuron 36 , 340–343 (2002).

Merlo, E. & Romano, A. Memory extinction entails the inhibition of the transcription factor NF-κB. PLoS ONE 3 , e3687 (2008).

Eisenhardt, D. & Menzel, R. Extinction learning, reconsolidation and the internal reinforcement hypothesis. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87 , 167–173 (2007).

Dudai, Y. & Eisenberg, M. Rites of passage of the engram: reconsolidation and the lingering consolidation hypothesis. Neuron 44 , 93–100 (2004).

Alberini, C. M. Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes? Trends Neurosci. 28 , 51–56 (2005).

Gordon, W. C. in Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms (eds Spear, N. E. & Kleim, J. A.) 319–339 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1981).

Mactutus, C. F., Riccio, D. C. & Ferek, J. M. Retrograde amnesia for old (reactivated) memory: some anomalous characteristics. Science 204 , 1319–1320 (1979).

Riccio, D. C., Moody, E. W. & Millin, P. M. Reconsolidation reconsidered. Integr. Physiol. Behav. Sci. 37 , 245–253 (2002).

Miller, R. R. & Marlin, N. A. in Memory Consolidation: Psychobiology of Cognition (eds Weingartner, H. & Parker, E. S.) 85–109 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1984).

Sara, S. J. Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learn. Mem. 7 , 73–84 (2000).

Nader, K., Hardt, O. & Wang, S. H. Response to Alberini: right answer, wrong question. Trends Neurosci. 28 , 346–347 (2005).

Blair, H. T., Schafe, G. E., Bauer, E. P., Rodrigues, S. M. & LeDoux, J. E. Synaptic plasticity in the lateral amygdala: a cellular hypothesis of fear conditioning. Learn. Mem. 8 , 229–242 (2001).

Schafe, G. E., Nader, K., Blair, H. T. & LeDoux, J. E. Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and molecular perspective. Trends Neurosci. 24 , 540–546 (2001).

von Hertzen, L. S. & Giese, K. P. Memory reconsolidation engages only a subset of immediate-early genes induced during consolidation. J. Neurosci. 25 , 1935–1942 (2005).

Biedenkapp, J. C. & Rudy, J. W. Context memories and reactivation: constraints on the reconsolidation hypothesis. Behav. Neurosci. 118 , 956–964 (2004).

Lewis, D. J. & Bregman, N. J. Source of cues for cue-dependent amnesia in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 85 , 421–426 (1973).

Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychol. Rev. 74 , 71–80 (1967).

Quartermain, D. & McEwen, B. S. Temporal characteristics of amnesia induced by protein synthesis inhibitor: determination by shock level. Nature 228 , 677–678 (1970).

Quartermain, D., McEwen, B. S. & Azmitia, E. C. Jr. Recovery of memory following amnesia in the rat and mouse. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 79 , 360–370 (1972).

Serota, R. G. Acetoxycycloheximide and transient amnesia in the rat. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 68 , 1249–1250 (1971).

Squire, L. R. & Barondes, S. H. Variable decay of memory and its recovery in cycloheximide-treated mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 69 , 1416–1420 (1972).

Cooper, R. M. & Koppenaal, R. J. Suppression and recovery of a one-trial avoidance response after a single ECS. Psychon. Sci. 1 , 303–304 (1964).

Kohlenberg, R. & Trabasso, T. O. M. Recovery of a conditioned emotional response after one or two electroconvulsive shocks. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 65 , 270–273 (1968).

Young, A. G. & Galluscio, E. H. Recovery from ECS-produced amnesia. Psychon. Sci. 22 , 149–151 (1971).

Berman, D. E. & Dudai, Y. Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex. Science 291 , 2417–2419 (2001).

Tronel, S., Milekic, M. H. & Alberini, C. M. Linking new information to a reactivated memory requires consolidation and not reconsolidation mechanisms. PLoS Biol. 3 , e293 (2005). This paper is one of the first to study the functional role of reconsolidation. It showed that reconsolidation was not necessary for learning an additional association after an initial association had been acquired.

Lee, J. L. Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning. Nature Neurosci. 11 , 1264–1266 (2008). This paper reported a functional difference between consolidation and reconsolidation, showing that strengthening an existing memory recruits reconsolidation but not consolidation mechanisms.

Ben Mamou, C., Gamache, K. & Nader, K. NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nature Neurosci. 9 , 1237–1239 (2006). The first paper to propose a framework that permits testing of the mechanisms that mediate transformation of a memory from a fixed to a labile state. It also showed that the mechanism that mediates freezing can be doubly dissociated from the mechanisms involved in initiating reconsolidation.

Suzuki, A., Mukawa, T., Tsukagoshi, A., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. Activation of LVGCCs and CB1 receptors required for destabilization of reactivated contextual fear memories. Learn. Mem. 15 , 426–433 (2008).

Ling, D. S. et al. Protein kinase Mζ is necessary and sufficient for LTP maintenance. Nature Neurosci. 5 , 295–296 (2002).

Serrano, P. et al. PKMζ maintains spatial, instrumental, and classically conditioned long-term memories. PLoS Biol. 6 , e318 (2008).

PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Lattal, K. M. & Abel, T. Different requirements for protein synthesis in acquisition and extinction of spatial preferences and context-evoked fear. J. Neurosci. 21 , 5773–5780 (2001).

Cammarota, M., Bevilaqua, L. R., Medina, J. H. & Izquierdo, I. Retrieval does not induce reconsolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Learn. Mem. 11 , 572–578 (2004).

Torras-Garcia, M., Lelong, J., Tronel, S. & Sara, S. J. Reconsolidation after remembering an odor-reward association requires NMDA receptors. Learn. Mem. 12 , 18–22 (2005).

Duvarci, S., Nader, K. & Ledoux, J. E. Activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinase-mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in the amygdala is required for memory reconsolidation of auditory fear conditioning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21 , 283–289 (2005).

Gruest, N., Richer, P. & Hars, B. Memory consolidation and reconsolidation in the rat pup require protein synthesis. J. Neurosci. 24 , 10488–10492 (2004).

Taubenfeld, S. M., Milekic, M. H., Monti, B. & Alberini, C. M. The consolidation of new but not reactivated memory requires hippocampal C/EBPβ. Nature Neurosci. 4 , 813–818 (2001).

Wang, S. H., Ostlund, S. B., Nader, K. & Balleine, B. W. Consolidation and reconsolidation of incentive learning in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 25 , 830–835 (2005).

Kelly, A., Laroche, S. & Davis, S. Activation of mitogen-activated protein kinase/extracellular signal-regulated kinase in hippocampal circuitry is required for consolidation and reconsolidation of recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 23 , 5354–5360 (2003).

Download references

Acknowledgements

K.N. was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the Canadian Foundation for Innovation and the Volkswagen, Alfred P. Sloan and EJLB Foundations; K.N. is a William Dawson chair and a E.W.R. Steacie Memorial Fellow; O.H. is supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. We would like to thank the Fondation des Treilles for hosting us while we wrote this manuscript. We thank C. Rankin, P. Frankland, E. Balaban and J. LeDoux for their comments on this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Psychology Department, McGill University, 1205 Doctor Penfield Avenue, Montreal, H3A 1B1, Quebec, Canada

Karim Nader & Oliver Hardt

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karim Nader .

Supplementary information

Supplementary information s1 (box).

Historical Perspectives (PDF 256 kb)

Related links

Further information.

Karim Nader's homepage

Oliver Hardt's homepage

Nature Reviews Neuroscience's Memory systems series

A Pavlovian conditioning paradigm in which an initially neutral stimulus (for example, a tone or the context in which the animals are conditioned) is paired with another stimulus that evokes pain or strong somatic discomfort (typically a footshock). After a single pairing the initially neutral stimulus will elicit a spectrum of fear-like or defensive responses.

(STM). Transient memory for an experience that does not require synthesis of new proteins or RNA and that can be expressed immediately. Typically, STM duration ranges from immediately to a couple of hours after acquisition.

(LTM). Relatively stable memory that develops over time and is assumed to be mediated by changes in synaptic efficacy. LTM depends on synthesis of new proteins and RNA. Typically it is tested one or more days after training, as it takes several hours to stabilize. Once stabilized it can last for the remainder of the animal's life.

(LTP). Traditionally demonstrated in hippocampal slice preparations, LTP is a persistent (lasting hours to days) enhancement of synaptic efficacy. It is rapidly induced by short high-frequency (tetanic) stimulation of a synaptic pathway.

(LTD). A persistent reduction of synaptic efficacy that can be induced by repeated low-frequency stimulation of a synaptic pathway. Maintenance of LTD might require de novo protein synthesis.

(PR-STM). By analogy with STM, PR-STM refers to a transient state into which existing LTM enters after it has been reactivated. The initial studies on reconsolidation indicate that PR-STM does not require synthesis of new RNA or proteins.

(PR-LTM). By analogy with LTM, PR-LTM refers to the period of stability that reactivated memory enters after completing reconsolidation.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Nader, K., Hardt, O. A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci 10 , 224–234 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2590

Download citation

Issue Date : March 2009

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2590

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Cellular mechanisms of contextual fear memory reconsolidation: role of hippocampal sfks, trkb receptors and glun2b-containing nmda receptors.

  • Eduarda G. Nachtigall
  • Jociane de C. Myskiw
  • Cristiane R. G. Furini

Psychopharmacology (2024)

Alcohol-specific transcriptional dynamics of memory reconsolidation and relapse

  • Koral Goltseker
  • Patricia Garay
  • Segev Barak

Translational Psychiatry (2023)

  • Axel Guskjolen
  • Mark S. Cembrowski

Molecular Psychiatry (2023)

Muscarinic receptor activation overrides boundary conditions on memory updating in a calcium/calmodulin-dependent manner

  • Cassidy E. Wideman
  • Andrew E. Huff
  • Boyer D. Winters

Neuropsychopharmacology (2023)

Modulation of methamphetamine memory reconsolidation by neural projection from basolateral amygdala to nucleus accumbens

  • Jia-Ying Li
  • Yang-Jung Yu

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Mechanisms of Memory Consolidation and Transformation

  • First Online: 10 February 2017

Cite this chapter

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

  • Melanie J. Sekeres 5 , 7 , 9 ,
  • Morris Moscovitch 5 , 6 , 7 &
  • Gordon Winocur 5 , 7 , 8 , 10  

Part of the book series: Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics ((SNPBE))

4159 Accesses

26 Citations

Memory consolidation is a dynamic process occurring over the lifetime of a memory, yet the underlying mechanisms are not well understood. The hippocampus is considered to be a critical structure for the acquisition, initial storage, and retrieval of a memory, but there is considerable debate over the continuing role of the hippocampus in representing a memory as it ages. Studies in rodents and humans both point towards a reorganization of hippocampus-dependent memory traces in the cortex over time, but when and how long it takes these large-scale network changes to occur is uncertain. In this chapter, we address how a memory that is initially dependent on the hippocampus becomes represented in the cortex, independently of the hippocampus. We also discuss how the quality of the memory changes (transforms) as the trace reorganizes over time, with a focus on hippocampal-cortical interactions as described by Trace Transformation Theory (TTT), and consider the degree to which evidence related to the mechanistic basis of memory consolidation in rodents applies to complex human memory. We conclude that theories like TTT provide a new approach to thinking about consolidation as an ongoing and interactive process involving the hippocampus, mPFC, and other brain regions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Neural circuits of memory consolidation and generalisation.

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Time-dependent memory transformation along the hippocampal anterior–posterior axis

consolidation hypothesis psychology definition

Retrieval of retrained and reconsolidated memories are associated with a distinct neural network

For this review, we will focus primarily on the type of memory that depends on the hippocampus for initial acquisition, including episodic memory in humans and episodic-like contextual and spatial memory in rodents.

Abbreviations

Anterior cingulate cortex

Cyclic-AMP response element binding protein

Immediate early gene

Late phase long-term potentiation

Medial prefrontal cortex

Medial temporal lobe

Multiple Trace Theory

Prefrontal cortex

Plasticity-related protein

Standard Consolidation Theory

Temporally-graded retrograde amnesia

Trace Transformation Theory

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Addis DR, Moscovitch M, Crawley AP, McAndrews MP (2004) Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus 14:752–762

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Alberini CM (2009) Transcription factors in long-term memory and synaptic plasticity. Physiol Rev 89:121–145

Alvarez P, Squire LR (1994) Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proc Natl Acad Sci 91:7041–7045

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Atherton LA, Dupret D, Mellor JR (2015) Memory trace replay: the shaping of memory consolidation by neuromodulation. Trends Neurosci 38:560–570

Bahrick HP (1984) Semantic memory content in permastore: fifty years of memory for Spanish learned in school. J Exp Psychol Gen 113:1–29

Bakker A, Kirwan CB, Miller M, Stark CE (2008) Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science 319(5870):1640–1642

Barco A, Marie H (2011) Genetic approaches to investigate the role of CREB in neuronal plasticity and memory. Mol Neurobiol 44:330–349

Barco A, Alarcon JM, Kandel ER (2002) Expression of constitutively active CREB protein facilitates the late phase of long-term potentiation by enhancing synaptic capture. Cell 108:689–703

Bartlett FC (1932) Remembering: an experiment in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, University Press, London

Google Scholar  

Beatty WW, Salmon DP, Bernstein N, Butters N (1987) Remote memory in a patient with amnesia due to hypoxia. Psychol Med 17:657–665

Ben-Yakov A, Dudai Y (2011) Constructing realistic engrams: poststimulus activity of hippocampus and dorsal striatum predicts subsequent episodic memory. J Neurosci 31:9032–9042

Bliss TV, Collingridge GL (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361:31–39

Bonnici HM, Chadwick MJ, Lutti A, Hassabis D, Weiskopf N, Maguire EA (2012) Detecting representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in vmPFC and hippocampus. J Neurosci 32:16982–16991

Bontempi B, Laurent-Demir C, Destrade C, Jaffard R (1999) Time-dependent reorganization of brain circuitry underlying long-term memory storage. Nature 400:671–675

Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ (1994) Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell 79:59–68

Burnham WH (1903) Retroactive amnesia: illustrative cases and a tentative explanation. Am J Psychol 118–132

Cai DJ, Aharoni D, Shuman T, Shobe J, Biane J, Song W, Wei B, Veshkini M, La-Vu M, Lou J et al (2016) A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature Adv Online Publ. doi: 10.1038/nature17955

Cairney SA, Durrant SJ, Musgrove H, Lewis PA (2011) Sleep and environmental context: interactive effects for memory. Exp Brain Res 214(1):83–92

Cajal SRY (1894) The croonian lecture: La fine structure des centres nerveux. Proc R Soc Lond 88:444–468

Article   Google Scholar  

Chan JC, LaPaglia JA (2013) Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110:9309–9313

Chan JC, Thomas AK, Bulevich JB (2009) Recalling a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility the reversed testing effect. Psychol Sci 20:66–73

Ciaramelli E (2008) The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in navigation: a case of impaired way finding and rehabilitation. Neuropsychologia 46(7):2099–2105

Ciaramelli E, Ghetti S (2007) What are confabulators’ memories made of? A study of subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulation. Neuropsychologia 45:1489–1500

Cole CJ, Mercaldo V, Restivo L, Yiu AP, Sekeres MJ, Han J-H, Vetere G, Pekar T, Ross PJ, Neve RL (2012) MEF2 negatively regulates learning-induced structural plasticity and memory formation. Nat Neurosci 15:1255–1264

Corkin S (2002) What’s new with the amnesic patient H.M.? Nat Rev Neurosci 3:153–160

Czajkowski R, Jayaprakash B, Wiltgen B, Rogerson T, Guzman-Karlsson MC, Barth AL, Trachtenberg JT, Silva AJ (2014) Encoding and storage of spatial information in the retrosplenial cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111:8661–8666

Dalla Barba G, La Corte V (2015) A neurophenomenological model for the role of the hippocampus in temporal consciousness. Evidence from confabulation. Frontiers Behav Neurosci 9

Davachi L, DuBrow S (2015) How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context. Trends Cogn Sci 19(2):92–99

Davis HP, Squire LR (1984) Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychol Bull 96:518–559

Debiec J, LeDoux JE, Nader K (2002) Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron 36:527–538

Denny CA, Kheirbek MA, Alba EL, Tanaka KF, Brachman RA, Laughman KB, Tomm NK, Turi GF, Losonczy A, Hen R (2014) Hippocampal memory traces are differentially modulated by experience, time, and adult neurogenesis. Neuron 83:189–201

Diekelmann S, Büchel C, Born J, Rasch B (2011) Labile or stable: opposing consequences for memory when reactivated during waking and sleep. Nat Neurosci 14:381–386

Dudai Y (2012) The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:227–247

Dudai Y, Karni A, Born J (2015) The consolidation and transformation of memory. Neuron 88:20–32

Dunsmoor JE, Murty VP, Davachi L, Phelps EA (2015) Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature 520:345–348

Einarsson EO, Nader K (2012) Involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation of recent and remote contextual fear memory. Learn Mem 19:449–452

Einarsson EO, Pors J, Nader K (2014) Systems reconsolidation reveals a selective role for the anterior cingulate cortex in generalized contextual fear memory expression. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:480–487

Finnie PS, Nader K (2012) The role of metaplasticity mechanisms in regulating memory destabilization and reconsolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1667–1707

Foster DJ, Wilson MA (2006) Reverse replay of Behavioral sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature 440:680–683

Frankland PW, Bontempi B (2005) The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:119–130

Frankland PW, Bontempi B, Talton LE, Kaczmarek L, Silva AJ (2004) The involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in remote contextual fear memory. Science 304:881–883

Frey U, Morris RG (1997) Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature 385:533–536

Frey U, Morris R (1998) Weak before strong: dissociating synaptic tagging and plasticity-factor accounts of late-LTP. Neuropharmacology 37:545–552

Fu M, Yu X, Lu J, Zuo Y (2012) Repetitive motor learning induces coordinated formation of clustered dendritic spines in vivo. Nature 483:92–95

Fujii T, Moscovitch M, Nadel L (2000) Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia, and the temporal lobe. Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd ed., vol 2. Memory and its disorders. Elsevier Science Publishers, pp 223–250

Furman O, Mendelsohn A, Dudai Y (2012) The episodic engram transformed: Time reduces retrieval-related brain activity but correlates it with memory accuracy. Learn Mem 19:575–587

Gallistel CR, Matzel LD (2013) The neuroscience of learning: beyond the Hebbian synapse. Annu Rev Psychol 64:169–200

Gelbard-Sagiv H, Mukamel R, Harel M, Malach R, Fried I (2008) Internally generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during free recall. Science 322(5898):96–101

Ghosh VE, Gilboa A (2014) What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53:104–114

Ghosh VE, Moscovitch M, Colella BM, Gilboa A (2014) Schema representation in patients with ventromedial PFC lesions. J Neurosci 34:12057–12070

Gilboa A, Moscovitch M, Baddeley A, Kopelman M, Wilson B (2002) The cognitive neuroscience of confabulation: a review and a model. Handb Mem Disord 2:315–342

Gilboa A, Winocur G, Grady CL, Hevenor SJ, Moscovitch M (2004) Remembering our past: functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events. Cereb Cortex 14:1214–1225

Gilboa A, Alain C, Stuss DT, Melo B, Miller S, Moscovitch M (2006) Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account. Brain 129:1399–1414

Goshen I, Brodsky M, Prakash R, Wallace J, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K (2011) Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell 147:678–689

Govindarajan A, Kelleher RJ, Tonegawa S (2006) A clustered plasticity model of long-term memory engrams. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:575–583

Greenberg ME, Ziff EB (1983) Stimulation of 3T3 cells induces transcription of the c-fos proto-oncogene. Nature 311(5985):433–438

Han JH, Kushner SA, Yiu AP, Cole CJ, Matynia A, Brown RA, Neve RL, Guzowski JF, Silva AJ, Josselyn SA (2007) Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science 316:457–460

Hardt O, Nader K, Nadel L (2013) Decay happens: the role of active forgetting in memory. Trends Cogn Sci 17(3):111–120

Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological approach. Wiley

Hebscher M, Barkan-Abramski M, Goldsmith M, Aharon-Peretz J, Gilboa A (2015) Memory, decision-making, and the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex (vmPFC): the roles of subcallosal and posterior orbitofrontal cortices in monitoring and control processes. Cerebral Cortex, bhv220

Hirshhorn M, Grady C, Rosenbaum RS, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2012) Brain regions involved in the retrieval of spatial and episodic details associated with a familiar environment: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 50:3094–3106

Howard MW, Kahana MJ (1999) Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 25(4):923

Hupbach A, Hardt O, Gomez R, Nadel L (2008) The dynamics of memory: context-dependent updating. Learn Mem 15:574–579

St. Jacques PL, Schacter DL (2013) Modifying memory selectively enhancing and updating personal memories for a museum tour by reactivating them. Psychol sci 24:537–543

St. Jacques PL, Olm C, Schacter DL (2013) Neural mechanisms of reactivation-induced updating that enhance and distort memory. Proc Nat Acad Sci 110:19671–19678

Josselyn SA, Shi C, Carlezon WA Jr, Neve RL, Nestler EJ, Davis M (2001) Long-term memory is facilitated by cAMP response element-binding protein overexpression in the amygdala. J Neurosci 21:2404–2412

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Josselyn SA, Köhler S, Frankland PW (2015) Finding the engram. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:521–534

Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR (2014) The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell 157:163–186

Kim JJ, Fanselow MS (1992) Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science 256:675–677

Kroes MC, Fernández G (2012) Dynamic neural systems enable adaptive, flexible memories. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1646–1666

Landauer TK (1975) Memory without organization: properties of a model with random storage and undirected retrieval. Cogn Psychol 7(4):495–531

Lashley KS (1950) In search of the engram. In: Society for experimental biology, symposium IV: physiological mechanisms in animal behavior, pp 454–482 (Academic Press)

Lee JQ, Zelinski EL, McDonald RJ, Sutherland RJ (2016) Heterarchic reinstatement of long-term memory: a concept on hippocampal amnesia in rodent memory research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71:154–166

Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser M-B, Moser EI (2007) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315(5814):961–966

Lewis PA, Durrant SJ (2011) Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends Cogn Sci 15(8):343–351

Ling DS, Benardo LS, Serrano PA, Blace N, Kelly MT, Crary JF, Sacktor TC (2002) Protein kinase Mζ is necessary and sufficient for LTP maintenance. Nat Neurosci 5:295–296

Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, Tonegawa S (2012) Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 484:381–385

Lonze BE, Ginty DD (2002) Function and regulation of CREB family transcription factors in the nervous system. Neuron 35:605–623

Lorenté de No R (1934) Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex II: continuation of the study if the ammonic system. J Psychol Neurol 46:113–177

Maguire EA (2014) Memory consolidation in humans: new evidence and opportunities. Exp Physiol 99:471–486

Maguire EA, Nannery R, Spiers HJ (2006) Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Brain 129:2894–2907

Manning JR, Polyn SM, Baltuch GH, Litt B, Kahana MJ (2011) Oscillatory patterns in temporal lobe reveal context reinstatement during memory search. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(31):12893–12897

Martin KC, Kandel ER (1996) Cell adhesion molecules, CREB, and the formation of new synaptic connections. Neuron 17:567–570

Maviel T, Durkin TP, Menzaghi F, Bontempi B (2004) Sites of neocortical reorganization critical for remote spatial memory. Science 305:96–99

McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev 102:419

Mendelsohn A, Furman O, Dudai Y (2010) Signatures of memory: brain coactivations during retrieval distinguish correct from incorrect recollection. Front Behav Neurosci 4:18

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Milner B, Corkin S, Teuber H-L (1968) Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of HM. Neuropsychologia 6:215–234

Misanin JR, Miller RR, Lewis DJ (1968) Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160:554–555

Morris R (1984) Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 11:47–60

Morris R, Garrud P, Rawlins J, O’Keefe J (1982) Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297:681–683

Moscovitch M (1989) Confabulation and the frontal systems: strategic versus associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In: Roediger HL, Craik FIM (eds) Varieties of memory and consciousness: essays in honour of Endel Tulving. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 133–160

Moscovitch M (1992) A neuropsychological model of memory and consciousness. Neuropsychol Mem 2:5–22

Moscovitch M (1995a) Confabulation. In: Schacter DL (Ed) Memory distortions: how minds, brains, and societies reconstruct the past. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, US, pp 226–251

Moscovitch M (1995b) Recovered consciousness: a hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 17:276–290

Moscovitch M, Winocur G (2002) The frontal cortex and working with memory. Principles of frontal lobe function, pp 188–209

Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Addis DR, Westmacott R, Grady C, McAndrews MP, Levine B, Black S, Winocur G, Nadel L (2005) Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat 207:35–66

Moscovitch M, Nadel L, Winocur G, Gilboa A, Rosenbaum RS (2006) The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:179–190

Moscovitch M (2007) Why the engram is elusive: memory does not exist until it is recovered. In: Tulving E, Roediger HL III, Dudai Y, Fitzpatrick SM (eds) Science of memory: concepts. Oxfod, UK: Oxford University Press, pp 17–22

Moscovitch M, Cabeza R, Winocur G, Nadel L (2016) Episodic memory and beyond: the hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annu Rev Psychol 67:105–134

Müller GE, Pilzecker A (1900) Experimentelle beiträge zur lehre vom gedächtniss (Vol 1). JA Barth

Nadel L, Moscovitch M (1997) Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:217–227

Nader K, Hardt O (2009) A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:224–234

Nader K, Schafe GE, Le Doux JE (2000) Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406:722–726

Nieuwenhuis IL, Takashima A (2011) The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory consolidation. Behav Brain Res 218:325–334

O’Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map, vol 3. Clarendon Press, Oxford

Paz R, Gelbard-Sagiv H, Mukamel R, Harel M, Malach R, Fried I (2010) A neural substrate in the human hippocampus for linking successive events. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(13):6046–6051

Penfield W, Milner B (1958) Memory deficit produced by bilateral lesions in the hippocampal zone. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 79:475–497

Ramirez S, Liu X, Lin P-A, Suh J, Pignatelli M, Redondo RL, Ryan TJ, Tonegawa S (2013) Creating a false memory in the hippocampus. Science 341:387–391

Restivo L, Tafi E, Ammassari-Teule M, Marie H (2009a) Viral-mediated expression of a constitutively active form of CREB in hippocampal neurons increases memory. Hippocampus 19:228–234

Restivo L, Vetere G, Bontempi B, Ammassari-Teule M (2009b) The formation of recent and remote memory is associated with time-dependent formation of dendritic spines in the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex. J Neurosci 29:8206–8214

Ribot TA (1882) Diseases of memory, an essay in the positive psychology, vol 43

Richards BA, Xia F, Santoro A, Husse J, Woodin MA, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (2014) Patterns across multiple memories are identified over time. Nat Neurosci 17:981–986

Ritchey M, Montchal ME, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2015) Delay-dependent contributions of medial temporal lobe regions to episodic memory retrieval. eLife 4, e05025

Rogerson T, Cai DJ, Frank A, Sano Y, Shobe J, Lopez-Aranda MF, Silva AJ (2014) Synaptic tagging during memory allocation. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:157–169

Rosenbaum RS, Priselac S, Kohler S, Black SE, Gao F, Nadel L, Moscovitch M (2000) Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nat Neurosci 3:1044–1048

Rosenbaum RS, McKinnon MC, Levine B, Moscovitch M (2004) Visual imagery deficits, impaired strategic retrieval, or memory loss: disentangling the nature of an amnesic person’s autobiographical memory deficit. Neuropsychologia 42:1619–1635

Rosenbaum RS, Kohler S, Schacter DL, Moscovitch M, Westmacott R, Black SE, Gao F, Tulving E (2005) The case of K.C.: contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theory. Neuropsychologia 43:989–1021

Rugg MD, Vilberg KL (2013) Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:255–260

Sadeh T, Ozubko JD, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2013) How we forget may depend on how we remember. Trends Cogn Sci 18(1):26–36

Sadeh T, Moran R, Goshen-Gottstein Y (2015) When items ‘pop into mind’: Variability in temporal-context reinstatement in free-recall. Psychon Bull Rev 22(3):779–790

Sadeh T, Ozubko JD, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2016) Forgetting patterns differentiate between two forms of memory representation. Psychol Sci 0956797616638307

Sanders HI, Warrington EK (1971) Memory for remote events in amnesic patients. Brain 94:661–668

Sano Y, Shobe JL, Zhou M, Huang S, Shuman T, Cai DJ, Golshani P, Kamata M, Silva AJ (2014) CREB regulates memory allocation in the insular cortex. Curr Biol 24:2833–2837

Sara SJ (2000) Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learn Mem 7:73–84

Schacter DL, Eich JE, Tulving E (1978) Richard Semon’s theory of memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 17:721–743

Schiller D, Monfils M-H, Raio CM, Johnson DC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA (2010) Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature 463:49–53

Schnider A (2008) The confabulating mind: how the brain creates reality. Oxford University Press, New York

Book   Google Scholar  

Scoville WB, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20:11–21

Sekeres MJ, Neve RL, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA (2010) Dorsal hippocampal CREB is both necessary and sufficient for spatial memory. Learn Mem 17:280–283

Sekeres MJ, Mercaldo V, Richards B, Sargin D, Mahadevan V, Woodin MA, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA (2012a) Increasing CRTC1 function in the dentate gyrus during memory formation or reactivation increases memory strength without compromising memory quality. J Neurosci 32:17857–17868

Sekeres MJ, Sargin D, Ross PJ, Josselyn SA (2012b) The role of CREB and CREB Co-activators in memory formation. Memory Mechanisms in Health and Disease: Mechanistic Basis of Memory pp 171–194 (World Scientific)

Sekeres MJ, Anderson JAE, Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Grady C (2015) The neural correlates of episodic memory transformation in humans. Soc Neurosci 2016, Abstract 18.03

Sekeres MJ, Bonasia K, St-Laurent M, Pishdadian S, Winocur G, Grady C, Moscovitch M (2016a) Recovering and preventing loss of detailed memory: differential rates of forgetting for detail types in episodic memory. Learn Memory 23:72–82

Sekeres MJ, Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Anderson JAE, Pishdadian S, Wojtowicz JM, St-Laurent M, McAndrews MP, Grady C (2016b) Changes in patterns of neural activity underlie a time-dependent transformation of memory across species (submitted)

Semon R (1923) Mnemic psychology (1909). George Allen and Unwin, Ltd, London

Sheldon S, Levine B (2013) Same as it ever was: vividness modulates the similarities and differences between the neural networks that support retrieving remote and recent autobiographical memories. Neuroimage 83:880–891

Shema R, Haramati S, Ron S, Hazvi S, Chen A, Sacktor TC, Dudai Y (2011) Enhancement of consolidated long-term memory by overexpression of protein kinase Mzeta in the neocortex. Science 331:1207–1210

Silva AJ, Zhou Y, Rogerson T, Shobe J, Balaji J (2009) Molecular and cellular approaches to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science 326:391–395

Squire LR (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 99:195–231

Squire LR (2004) Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82:171–177

Squire LR, Bayley PJ (2007) The neuroscience of remote memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17:185–196

Squire LR, Wixted JT (2011) The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since HM. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:259

Steinvorth S, Levine B, Corkin S (2005) Medial temporal lobe structures are needed to re-experience remote autobiographical memories: evidence from H.M. and W.R. Neuropsychologia 43:479–496

St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, Levine B, McAndrews MP (2009) Determinants of autobiographical memory in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy or excisions. Neuropsychologia 47:2211–2221

St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, Jadd R, McAndrews MP (2014) The perceptual richness of complex memory episodes is compromised by medial temporal lobe damage. Hippocampus 24:560–576

St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, McAndrews MP (2016) The retrieval of perceptual memory details depends on right hippocampal integrity and activation. Cortex (under review)

Takashima A, Nieuwenhuis IL, Jensen O, Talamini LM, Rijpkema M, Fernández G (2009) Shift from hippocampal to neocortical centered retrieval network with consolidation. J Neurosci 29:10087–10093

Tayler KK, Tanaka KZ, Reijmers LG, Wiltgen BJ (2013) Reactivation of neural ensembles during the retrieval of recent and remote memory. Curr Biol 23:99–106

Teixeira CM, Pomedli SR, Maei HR, Kee N, Frankland PW (2006) Involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in the expression of remote spatial memory. J Neurosci 26:7555–7564

Teng E, Squire LR (1999) Memory for places learned long ago is intact after hippocampal damage. Nature 400:675–677

Thorndyke PW (1977) Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cogn Psychol 9:77–110

Tonegawa S, Pignatelli M, Roy DS, Ryan TJ (2015) Memory engram storage and retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol 35:101–109

Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, Bethus I, Spooner PA, Wood ER, Witter MP, Morris RG (2007) Schemas and memory consolidation. Science 316:76–82

Tse D, Takeuchi T, Kakeyama M, Kajii Y, Okuno H, Tohyama C, Bito H, Morris RG (2011) Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science 333:891–895

Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. Organization of memory. Academic, London 381(4)

van Kesteren MT, Ruiter DJ, Fernández G, Henson RN (2012) How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci 35(4):211–219

van Kesteren MT, Beul SF, Takashima A, Henson RN, Ruiter DJ, Fernández G (2013) Differential roles for medial prefrontal and medial temporal cortices in schema-dependent encoding: from congruent to incongruent. Neuropsychologia 51(12):2352–2359

Varela C, Weiss S, Meyer R, Halassa M, Biedenkapp J, Wilson MA, Goosens KA, Bendor D (2016) Tracking the time-dependent role of the hippocampus in memory recall using DREADDs. PLoS ONE 11:e0154374

Vetere G, Restivo L, Cole CJ, Ross PJ, Ammassari-Teule M, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (2011) Spine growth in the anterior cingulate cortex is necessary for the consolidation of contextual fear memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108:8456–8460

Vetere G, Kenney JW, Tran L, Wheeler A, Josselyn S, Frankland PW (2015) In vivo and in silico interrogation of a fear memory network. Soc Neurosci 2016, Abstract 534.17

Viskontas IV, Carr VA, Engel SA, Knowlton BJ (2009) The neural correlates of recollection: hippocampal activation declines as episodic memory fades. Hippocampus 19:265–272

Vousden DA, Epp J, Okuno H, Nieman BJ, van Eede M, Dazai J, Ragan T, Bito H, Frankland PW, Lerch JP (2015) Whole-brain mapping of Behaviorally induced neural activation in mice. Brain Struct Funct 220:2043–2057

Wang S-H, Morris RG (2010) Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation. Annu Rev Psychol 61:49–79

Wang S-H, Redondo RL, Morris RG (2010) Relevance of synaptic tagging and capture to the persistence of long-term potentiation and everyday spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107:19537–19542

Wheeler AL, Teixeira CM, Wang AH, Xiong X, Kovacevic N, Lerch JP, McIntosh AR, Parkinson J, Frankland PW (2013) Identification of a functional connectome for long-term fear memory in mice. PLoS Comput Biol 9:e1002853

Wiltgen BJ, Silva AJ (2007) Memory for context becomes less specific with time. Learn Mem 14:313–317

Winocur G (1990) Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or dorsomedial thalamic lesions. Behav Brain Res 38:145–154

Winocur G, Mills JA (1970) Transfer between related and unrelated problems following hippocampal lesions in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 73(1):162

Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2011) Memory transformation and systems consolidation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17:766–780

Winocur G, Weiskrantz L (1976) An investigation of paired-associate learning in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 14(1):97–110

Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Caruana DA, Binns MA (2005a) Retrograde amnesia in rats with lesions to the hippocampus on a test of spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 43(11):1580–1590

Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Fogel S, Rosenbaum RS, Sekeres M (2005b) Preserved spatial memory after hippocampal lesions: effects of extensive experience in a complex environment. Nat Neurosci 8:273–275

Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Sekeres M (2007) Memory consolidation or transformation: context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory. Nat Neurosci 10:555–557

Winocur G, Frankland PW, Sekeres M, Fogel S, Moscovitch M (2009) Changes in context-specificity during memory reconsolidation: selective effects of hippocampal lesions. Learn Mem 16:722–729

Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Sekeres M (2010) An investigation of the effects of hippocampal lesions in rats on pre- and postoperatively acquired spatial memory in a complex environment. Hippocampus 20:1350–1365

Winocur G, Becker S, Luu P, Rosenzweig S, Wojtowicz JM (2012) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and memory interference. Behav Brain Res 227:464–469

Winocur G, Sekeres MJ, Binns MA, Moscovitch M (2013) Hippocampal lesions produce both nongraded and temporally graded retrograde amnesia in the same rat. Hippocampus, 23(5):330–341

Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, Tobin WF, Zweig JA, Tennant K, Jones T, Zuo Y (2009) Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature 462:915–919

Ye L, Allen WE, Thompson KR, Tian Q, Hsueh B, Ramakrishnan C et al.(2016). Wiring and molecular features of frefrontal ensembles representing distinct experiences. Cell (in press)

Zhou Y, Won J, Karlsson MG, Zhou M, Rogerson T, Balaji J, Neve R, Poirazi P, Silva AJ (2009) CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat Neurosci 12:1438–1443

Zola-Morgan SM, Squire LR (1990) The primate hippocampal formation: evidence for a time-limited role in memory storage. Science 250:288–290

Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral D (1986) Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 6:2950–2967

Download references

Acknowledgements

The preparation of this chapter was supported by a grant to MM and GW from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (CIHR—grant # MGP 6694). Research by the authors reported in this paper was supported by CIHR grant MGP 6694 and by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council to GW (# A8181) and MM (# A8347). MJS was supported by a CIHR post-doctoral fellowship.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest, Toronto, ON, M6A 2E1, Canada

Melanie J. Sekeres, Morris Moscovitch & Gordon Winocur

Department of Psychology, Baycrest, Toronto, ON, M6A 2E1, Canada

Morris Moscovitch

Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, M5S 3G3, Canada

Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, M5S 3G3, Canada

Gordon Winocur

Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Baylor University, Waco, TX, 76798, USA

Melanie J. Sekeres

Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, ON, K9J 7B8, Canada

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Melanie J. Sekeres , Morris Moscovitch or Gordon Winocur .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Ruhr-University Bochum, Bochum, Germany

Nikolai Axmacher

University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland

Björn Rasch

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Sekeres, M.J., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G. (2017). Mechanisms of Memory Consolidation and Transformation. In: Axmacher, N., Rasch, B. (eds) Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation. Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45066-7_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45066-7_2

Published : 10 February 2017

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-45064-3

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-45066-7

eBook Packages : Behavioral Science and Psychology Behavioral Science and Psychology (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Memory Consolidation: Definition And Examples In Psychology

In memory consolidation psychology, memory and memory consolidation rely on a healthy body and brain. Many areas may affect your mind's ability to retain information, including age, substance use, medications, diseases, and mental health. Learning about these areas can be beneficial when trying to improve your memory consolidation ability.

Memory consolidation: Reinforcing memory traces

The memory consolidation process involves the storage of information in long-term memory. This occurs when memory traces are reinforced, stabilized, and saved so that we can later recall them. 

What is a memory trace?

Also called an engram, a memory trace is a change in the nervous system that is thought to encode a memory. These memory traces assist us in reconstructing memories. After the memory trace is created, consolidation strengthens it through changes in neural pathways. The exact alterations that occur to create a memory trace are unknown; but research suggests that they are formed in the cerebellum and hippocampus . 

How consolidation occurs

To understand the concept of memory consolidation, it may be helpful to think of memory as a muscle. Exercising your memory with repetition may help you retain memories over time and feel confident in recalling information. For example, perhaps you spent hours and hours in school studying and using the Pythagorean theorem. Doing so may have allowed you to exercise your brain to store the formula in your long-term memory. This process is called  memory consolidation . 

On the opposite end, placing your keys on the kitchen table when you get home and forgetting about them can be an example of a lack of memory consolidation. In this case, placing the keys down is an automatic gesture, so you may forget that you've done it. However, if you always place your keys on the same hook, your memory consolidation may allow you to know where they are when you leave the next day.

What are memories?

Memory is defined as the process of receiving information, storing details, and recalling that information later, all within the mind. Memory involves many portions of the brain, but the hippocampus, found within the brain's temporal lobes, is primarily  responsible for emotions and memory . 

This pattern of reactivation is a memory. Rather than memory being an organized system with separate compartments in the brain, it is an ever-changing system of neural patterns and pathways in which information travels across many brain regions.

In memory consolidation, recalling the same piece of information repeatedly trains the neurons to act together efficiently and rapidly. As this repetition continues, the neurons learn to fire together in the original pattern; thus, the information is more readily available in memory.

Theories of forgetting 

For decades, experts in human memory have sought to explain the processes by which we experience memory failures. Theories of forgetting can help us understand how deficiencies in memory retrieval occur. While several other frameworks exist (e.g., repressed memories), the following are five of the most prominent theories of forgetting, separated by whether they impact short-term or long-term memory. 

Forgetting information in short-term memory

Trace decay theory of forgetting .

An early theory of forgetting, the trace decay theory, posits that failing to retrieve a memory trace can cause it to deteriorate. According to the trace decay theory, all memories fade over time. When old memories decay, their retrieval becomes more difficult. 

Displacement theory of forgetting

The displacement theory of forgetting states that the storage of new memories in short-term memory causes older information to be removed. This displacement occurs because of limited space in short-term memory. 

Forgetting information in long-term memory 

Retrieval failure theory of forgetting .

According to the retrieval failure theory, forgetting occurs when information in long-term memory is unable to be recalled. Proponents of this theory state that our memories are associated with certain memory cues, or retrieval cues. A retrieval cue may be a related image, word, sound, or other stimulus. When these retrieval cues are unavailable, we are not able to retrieve the information, and we experience a form of memory failure known as cue-dependent forgetting.

Interference theory of forgetting

The interference theory of forgetting is based on the idea that memories can be lost due to the formation or existence of other memories. These recent and remote memories can impact memory performance through two processes—proactive and retroactive interference. Proactive interference occurs when existing memories affect our ability to create new memories. Retroactive interference occurs when the formation of new memories interferes with our retrieval of existing memories. 

Consolidation theory of forgetting

Focused on disturbances during the process of reinforcing memory traces, the consolidation theory of forgetting is associated with the physiological, instead of psychological, functioning of the memory system. According to the consolidation theory, interference with the consolidation process prevents memory traces from stabilizing, making retrieval more difficult. There are several ways consolidation can be disrupted, including medications, brain abnormalities, and lack of sleep. Consolidation theory has been backed by functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.

Causes of memory challenges

Before understanding memory consolidation, it may benefit you to understand the causes of memory challenges and memory loss that may cause difficulty with cognitive ability. 

Age-related memory loss

As humans age, it can be natural for memory function to diminish gradually. Knowing that memories are constantly changing neural patterns, it can be easy to understand why no memories are permanent, including long-term and stored memories. Memory consolidation psychology shows many possible reasons that memory can decrease as people age. However, the two most common causes include reduced blood flow to the brain and deterioration of the hippocampus.

Other cases of memory decline can occur on a much more rapid and severe scale from certain diseases such as Alzheimer's. Alzheimer's disease is a form of dementia that progresses and irreversibly reduces an individual's mental functioning, including memory. Alzheimer's disease often leads to needing full-time care in the later stages of the progression. Dementia is a general term describing symptoms that significantly alter mental functioning.

Mental illness

Memory can be a vital part of personality and daily functioning. A few mental illnesses that impact cognitive ability may also lead to poor memory, including depression and anxiety. Those experiencing depression often report feeling as if their mind is foggy or clouded. When the brain is preoccupied with rumination and feelings of worry and anxiety, it can impair one's ability to form memories and learn new information. 

Schizophrenia is another mental illness commonly associated with decreased memory function. One meta-analysis documented  significant memory impairment in schizophrenia . According to the study, "the impairment was stable, wide-ranging, and not substantially affected by potential moderating factors such as the severity of psychopathology and duration of illness."

Substance use 

Substance use may also cause  significant interferences  with memory consolidation and recall. Alcohol is one substance known for its potential memory interference, as it can lead to partial memory loss or complete blackouts if consumed in significant amounts. 

Research is still being conducted on the ways that alcohol interferes with memory, but it is believed to be, in part, due to the disruptions that alcohol creates in the hippocampus. It is also shown that alcohol use interferes with neurons and their ability to respond to signals from other cells consistently. Benzodiazepines, often prescribed for acute anxiety, also have similar impacts on memory consolidation. This effect is often amplified in those who misuse benzodiazepines.

How to improve memory consolidation 

According to memory consolidation psychology, the brain thrives on stimulation. Therefore, it is often said that learning new skills effectively cultivates brain health. For example, learning to play a musical instrument or speak a new language can exercise your brain and activate multiple areas.  

Physical exercise is another proven way to boost mental functioning. The University of British Columbia conducted a study showing that  regular aerobic exercise  seemingly increased hippocampus size in older women with possible cognitive impairment. In addition, chronic inflammation can be linked to memory loss and cognitive decline later in life, and regular physical activity is linked to reducing inflammation in the body. 

The  quality of sleep  you get is also associated with brain functioning and memory. Getting enough sleep can allow you to better process, consolidate, and store new information as memories in the brain. Memory loss or difficulty remembering information is also often associated with sleep deprivation, sleep apnea, and other sleep-related disorders such as narcolepsy. 

Counseling options 

Experiencing difficulties with processing and retaining memory can be unsettling and, in some cases, frightening. If you are struggling with memory loss or other concerns about cognitive function, speak to a doctor immediately. A neurologist can help you isolate the potential causes of your symptoms and begin treatment if necessary.

If you discover that your memory issues could be related to a mental health condition like depression or anxiety, contacting a therapist may also be beneficial. A psychologist can use methods like cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) to isolate the root cause of your challenges and provide you with tools to cultivate a healthier state of mind and a more significant capacity for memory retention.

The rise of online therapy in treating anxiety, depression, and trauma-related disorders has removed many common barriers to therapy that keep people from getting help. For instance, online therapy is often more convenient, flexible, and affordable than in-person therapy. Studies have backed up these claims, showcasing the  cost-effectiveness  of internet-based interventions. 

Online platforms like  BetterHelp match clients with mental health professionals with experience in various specialized care. You can speak with a counselor via phone, text, video chat, or online messaging to get started as you move toward better mental health.  

  • What Is Insight? Definition, Psychology, And Practical Examples Medically reviewed by Kimberly L Brownridge , LPC, NCC, BCPC
  • Understanding Prejudice: Origins, Mental Health Impact, And Ways To Combat Bias & Discrimination Medically reviewed by Nikki Ciletti , M.Ed, LPC
  • Psychologists
  • Relationships and Relations

Study.com

In order to continue enjoying our site, we ask that you confirm your identity as a human. Thank you very much for your cooperation.

Warning: The NCBI web site requires JavaScript to function. more...

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Bermúdez-Rattoni F, editor. Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes to Brain Imaging. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007.

Cover of Neural Plasticity and Memory

Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes to Brain Imaging.

Chapter 11 memory reconsolidation or updating consolidation.

Carlos J. Rodriguez-Ortiz and Federico Bermúdez-Rattoni .

11.1. INTRODUCTION

For a long time, consolidation was seen as a process achieved only on newly acquired memories aimed to store them for the long term. However, pioneer and recent studies have demonstrated that after retrieval, long-term memories may once more undergo a consolidation-like process referred to as reconsolidation. Mainly, reconsolidation is sustained by the now widely reported observation that after a memory trace is activated by means of retrieval and is susceptible to disruption by the same treatments that disrupt memory during consolidation. However, the functional purpose of this process is still a matter of debate.

Recent evidence indicates that reconsolidation is indeed a process by which updated information is integrated through the synthesis of proteins to a memory trace. Hence, the so-called reconsolidation seems more like an updating consolidation intended to modify retrieved memory by a process that integrates updated experience into long-term memory. Through this process, previously consolidated memory is partially destabilized. By the infusion of disrupting agents, it appears as if the process is intended to consolidate memory again. In this chapter, we discuss this issue and propose that updating consolidation is a more descriptive term for this process.

11.2. CONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS

The classification of memories according to their duration was initiated by Hermann Ebbinghauss in his work titled “Uber das Gadachtnis” (About Memory) and formalized later by William James. From these works it appears that memory has, on the basis of its time course, at least two forms, namely short- and long-term memories. Although no fixed time span segregates these two memory forms, it is clear that information stored in long-term memory (LTM) undergoes a consolidation process that strengthens it over time into a stable memory trace.

This process does not take place for short-term memory (STM), which decays much sooner. The term consolidation is acknowledged to Müller and Pilzecker on their study reported in 1900. In one set of experiments, they trained subjects to memorize a list of paired syllables. On the test day, cue syllables (each one was a single syllable of a pair) were presented and the number of complementary recalled syllables was used as a measure of memory retention. A reduction in the number of retrieved syllables from the first list was observed if a second (distracting) list of syllables was presented shortly after training. Furthermore, the longer the interval between the two lists, the less the performance was affected. The researchers concluded that the second list interfered in a time-dependent manner on a physiological process that accounts for the strengthening of memories. They named this process “consolidirung.” 1

These observations were mostly ignored until Duncan reported almost 50 years later that he proved that an electroconvulsive shock (ECS) applied after training disrupted memory. Moreover, he showed that memory disruption correlated with the interval between training and ECS application. Since ECS was longer spaced in time from training, memory impairment was reduced. Since then, several other researchers have shown that interfering treatments — from ECS to intracerebral microinjections of protein synthesis inhibitors — applied after acquisition prevent LTM storage. Consistently, LTM is not affected if the intrusive treatment is applied outside the vulnerability window. This, along with the consolidation hypothesis, led to the idea that memory undergoes this time-dependent stabilization process only once. Reliability among a huge amount of related studies sustained the prominent place that this idea occupies in the current model of consolidation. 2 , 3

An important transition in memory research took place after Hebb’s dual-trace proposal suggesting that memory is at first in a labile state maintained by a reverberating neural ensemble. LTM arises from cellular changes in this ensemble allowing memory stabilization. 4 Although it is still matter of intense debate whether STM and LTM are serial or parallel processes, dual-trace theory stressed the weight that cellular entities have in memory processing, turning research to the cellular events underlying memory. At the cellular level, STM undergoes activation of transduction * cascades (mainly kinase pathways) after neuronal stimulation. It is proposed that STM remains as long as these cascades are active but for LTM, transduction signals are carried to the nucleus where transcription * is achieved. Afterward, RNA translation † will ultimately lead to protein synthesis. These proteins account for cellular plastic changes that are considered the cellular correlations of stable LTM traces, i.e., they are considered the cellular counterparts of consolidation. Hence, memory consolidation requires protein synthesis. It has been extensively reported that protein synthesis inhibition disrupts LTM without affecting STM. 3 , 7–9

11.3. RECONSOLIDATION ERA

As noted earlier, consolidation was seen as a process achieved only on newly acquired memories with the intent of long-term storage. However, pioneer studies indicated that consolidated memories may undergo a consolidation-like process more than once under certain conditions. In 1968, Misanin et al. 10 habituated rats to lick from a drinking bottle in a conditioning chamber, after which they were trained in a fear conditioning task in which a tone (conditioned stimulus, CS) was paired to a footshock (unconditioned stimulus, US). As a result, a conditioned response was obtained and used as a measure of memory, in this case, a reduced licking rate from the water bottle after the tone onset. They reported that an ECS applied immediately after conditioning disrupted memory consolidation ( Figure 11.1b , Group 2). The interesting point arose from Group 3. Those animals were trained but without delivery of an ECS. A day later, the consolidated fear memory was reactivated by presenting the tone again. Immediately after this memory reactivation, an ECS was applied with the surprising result that memory was impaired when tested 24 hours later ( Figure 11.1b , Group 3). Notably, ECS was unable to disrupt memory if the tone cue was not presented ( Figure 11.1b , Group 4) and the phenomenon was referred as cue-dependent amnesia. 10

FIGURE 11.1

Data from first report on retrograde amnesia induced after memory reactivation. (a) Schematic representation of protocol used by Misanin et al. (b) Group 1 shows fear conditioning behavior displayed under this protocol, measured as reduced licking from (more...)

Even though these results were at first not replicated, 11 they encouraged further (mainly unnoticed) work on the possibility that consolidated memories enter into an active stage upon retrieval. For example, Gordon showed that as occurs with newly acquired memories, retrieved memories are susceptible to disruption in a time-dependent manner. 12 Cue-dependent amnesia was further studied in the active–inactive memory model proposed by Lewis. 13 who claimed that memories become active under two conditions: when newly acquired and when reactivated by means of retrieval. Any other memory is in an inactive stable state. Recently, cue-dependent amnesia was taken up again and is now referred as reconsolidation.

Reconsolidation proposes that after a memory trace is activated by means of retrieval, it is susceptible to disruption by the same treatments that disrupt memory during consolidation. 14 , 15 In 1992, Bucherelli and Tassoni 16 reported that inactivation of the parabrachial nuclei by infusions of tetrodotoxin disrupted previously consolidated memories when reactivated. Similarly, Susan Sara’s group reported that infusions of either NMDA or β-adrenergic antagonists (which disrupted LTM when applied after training) disrupted a clearly established memory trace upon retrieval. 17–19 Since then, memory reconsolidation has actively been studied.

The most acknowledged study is the one carried out by Nader and coworkers in 2000. 20 This work brought general attention to the reconsolidation phenomenon because of the clean data reported and because of the use of a translational inhibitor that interfered with protein synthesis, considered to be the main cellular substrate for memory consolidation. The experiments were performed in the widely studied fear conditioning task and showed that the same treatment applied under circumstances that disrupt consolidation also impairs memory after retrieval. Similar to the report by Misanin and coworkers, Nader et al. conditioned rats in a tone-foot-shock association but memory was assessed by the percentage of the time that rats were immobile (except for movements required for breathing) to the total time the tone was presented (freezing). The day after conditioning, the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin was injected in the amygdala after the tone presentation.

When the subjects were tested 24 hours later, they performed poorly compared to the rats that were not anisomycin-injected ( Figure 11.2b ). The same treatment was unable to disrupt memory if a retrieval session was not performed ( Figure 11.2b , Group 3). The researchers also showed that the effects of anisomycin were time-dependent. When injected 6 hours after memory reactivation, it is unable to disrupt memory. In the years following the Nader study, a wide variety of reports have shown that reconsolidation is indeed a general process achieved in different species and different kinds of memories. 21–30

FIGURE 11.2

Intraamygdalar infusion of a protein-synthesis blocker disrupts consolidated fear memory. (a) Schematic representation of protocol used by Nader et al. (b) Group 1 shows fear conditioning behavior displayed under this protocol, measured as high percentage (more...)

11.4. ON RESTRAINTS OF RECONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS

Despite the huge body of experiments supporting reconsolidation, some did not uncover consolidated memory susceptibility to disruption after retrieval. 31–34 However, some recent reports have helped explain to an extent why a reconsolidation process is not revealed under certain protocols. 26 , 35 , 36 To address this issue, we must first look to what is called extinction and again take up the conditioning protocol on which a great number of memory tasks rely. On conditioning, a CS, like a tone, is associated to an US, like a foot shock. As a result, the CS elicits a response that is used as a measure of memory, like freezing.

However, CS presentation in the absence of US eventually leads to a response decrement; in our example, animals stopped freezing. This is extinction. On extinction, the CS is now associated to no-US. Like any other learning, extinction undergoes consolidation. To assess reconsolidation, the CS is commonly presented as a retrieval cue that may lead to extinction. During testing, treatments applied on retrieval may reflect effects over the CS-US association in which case disruption of the conditioned response is observed (reconsolidation is uncovered).

On the other hand, treatments may impair consolidation of extinction, in which case the CS-US association seems unaltered. On this latter scenario, results may be interpreted as lack of a reconsolidation process. Hence, studies like Vianna et al. 32 and Berman and Dudai 31 reported that protein synthesis inhibition disrupted extinction, leaving CS-US association unimpaired or even strengthened, and pointing at the impression that reconsolidation does not occur under these protocols.

Pedreira and Maldonado 35 offered evidence to move forward using a contextual memory task in crabs. When crabs are placed in a particular context and an object is passed overhead, they escape from the moving object, but when this stimulus is repeated several times, the crabs freeze upon presentation of the passing object. However, when the context is changed, freezing of the crabs does not take place. Thus, the context is associated to the passing object and freezing is used as a measure of memory. To induce extinction, the animals were exposed to the context in the absence of the passing object. Pedreira and Maldonado placed conditioned crabs in the training context for either 5 or 60 min as a retrieval session. During the session, they systemically applied the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide and tested 24 hours later. Crabs exposed for 5 min did not undergo a clear extinction and when tested, effects over reconsolidation were found. Conversely, crabs exposed for 60 min extinguished the conditioned response and when tested, extinction was impaired.

These findings have been replicated by many others. 26 , 36 Eisenberg et al. 26 trained rats in a taste aversion task. Task acquisition was achieved by pairing a taste with an intraperitoneal injection of a visceral malaise-inducing agent (LiCl). Taste–malaise association produced a long-term aversive memory observed by a reduced intake of that taste in a second presentation compared to its consumption on acquisition. However, on the third presentation, intake was increased, showing that the aversive memory was extinguished. Protein synthesis inhibition disrupted extinction when applied on the second taste presentation leaving CS-US pairing unaltered, i.e., a failing to detect a reconsolidation process.

However, when rats were subjected to the taste–malaise association for two consecutive sessions, extinction was not observed on the subsequent presentations. Under these conditions, protein synthesis inhibition on the presentation following the association sessions showed aversion impairment when the animals were tested, i.e., reconsolidation was revealed. In the same study, medaka fishes were trained in a fear conditioning task. Consistent with the results obtained from rats, protein synthesis inhibition impairs consolidation on the session that led to extinction affected and, in the absence of extinction, protein synthesis inhibition impaired CS-US reconsolidation. Thus, when consolidation of extinction memory was initiated on the retrieval session, reconsolidation of the CS-US association was not observed.

Other authors have found that pharmacological treatments disrupted LTM of recently consolidated but not older consolidated memories. 37 , 38 That is, when the retrieval session takes place on the days following acquisition, memory is susceptible to consolidation blockers. However, as the retrieval session is spaced in time from training, memory becomes less sensitive to these blockers. These results point to the idea that reconsolidation is a process achieved only by recently consolidated memories upon retrieval. However, Suzuki and co-workers 36 reported that stronger and older memories are susceptible to disruption upon retrieval too. They showed that stronger and older memories need of a longer retrieval trial to be disrupted by the blockade of protein synthesis than weaker and younger memories. Consistent with the reports of Pedreira and Maldonado 35 and Eisenberg et al., 26 these effects were found as long as the retrieval trial did not lead to extinction. Therefore, it seems that the strength of the reminder is related to memory susceptibility to disruption after retrieval.

11.5. CONSOLIDATION AND RECONSOLIDATION: THE SAME PROCESS?

Probably the most important question regarding the reconsolidation process is: why and under what circumstances is reconsolidation attained? At first glance, it seems counterintuitive to carry out an already achieved process again, i.e., to consolidate once more an already consolidated memory, as is implied by the reconsolidation term. It has been reported that some of the molecular mechanisms involved in consolidation are also required for reconsolidation of the same memory trace and in the same brain region. 20 , 23 , 24 , 26 , 39–43 For example, particular transcription factors have been proven necessary for both consolidation and reconsolidation processes in different memory tasks. Kida and colleagues 40 showed CREB involvement in contextual fear conditioning * memory in mice. Also in mice, Bozon and co-workers 41 reported a zif268 requirement in object recognition memory † and finally, Merlo et al. 42 showed NF-B participation in contextual memory using the crab model described above. In rats, Duvarci, Nader, and LeDoux 43 showed that the extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) pathway must be activated in the amygdala for both consolidation and reconsolidation of fear conditioning. Furthermore Sangha et al. 24 reported that for the Lymnaea stagnalis snail, consolidation and reconsolidation occurred in the same cell. These data indicate that reconsolidation may be a remaking of the consolidation process. 15

However, several other studies suggest that consolidation and reconsolidation are different processes. Taubenfeld and colleagues 44 reported that the transcription factor C/EBPβ is needed for consolidation but not for consolidation of a context-dependent task in the dorsal hippocampus. Tronel and Sara 45 described differential activation of several brain regions after retrieval compared to consolidation of an odor-reward task learning analyzed by c/Fos immunohistochemistry. In the same regard, Kelly and co-workers 28 demonstrated an increase in phosphorylation of ERK kinases in the dentate gyrus and the entorhinal cortex after training in an object recognition task along with increased phosphorylation in the hippocampal CA1 region and the entorhinal cortex after memory retrieval. On taste memory, it was reported that muscarinic receptor activity in the gustatory cortex is required for safe memory consolidation but not for postretrieval consolidation. 46 Similarly, protein synthesis in the central amygdala is required for consolidation but not for reconsolidation of conditioned taste aversion. 47 Finally, Lee et al. 48 reported that the growth factor BDNF is required for consolidation but not for reconsolidation, and transcription factor zif268 is needed for reconsolidation but not consolidation in the same brain region and memory task. All this evidence discards the possibility that reconsolidation is a recapitulation of consolidation but does not solve the problem. The question remains: what is the physiological purpose of reconsolidation?

11.6. RECONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS RECONSIDERED: UPDATING CONSOLIDATION PROPOSAL

Early and recent reviews suggest that reconsolidation may be a state for incoming information to modify established memories but experimental support is almost completely absent. 13 , 14 , 49 , 50 However, our group recently reported that newly acquired and retrieved taste recognition memory is susceptible to disruption by the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin when applied in the insular cortex (IC), a proven site for taste memory consolidation. In that work, the attenuation of neophobia (AN) task was used. Animals showed graded increases in intake after repeated presentations of the same tastant until a plateau was reached ( Figure 11.3a ). 51 , 52

FIGURE 11.3

Attenuation of neophobia (AN) behavior and protein synthesis inhibition effect (a) Mean ± S.E.M. intake (in mL) of 0.3% saccharin solution on unoperated rats. Taste presentations were daily for 15 min. (b) and (c) Anisomycin infusion in insular (more...)

Importantly, anisomycin injections produced a partial disruption of previously consolidated memory and the observed impairment became less noticeable as a response plateau was reached ( Figure 11.3b and c ). On asymptotic performance, anisomycin affects no longer consolidated memory ( Figure 11.3d ). These results led to the proposal that a protein-synthesis-dependent process is achieved as long as updated experience capable of affecting behavior is acquired. This process is aimed to integrate updated relevant information to LTM. Consistently, part of the older consolidated memory is dependent on protein synthesis. Partial susceptibility to disruption of a previously consolidated memory trace may be the physiological substrate that allows incoming material to integrate to memory.

Furthermore, when there is no more relevant information to be learned, i.e., after asymptotic task performance is reached, memory is no longer vulnerable to protein synthesis inhibition. Moreover, when the AN plateau has been reached and information of a different quality is provided, like aversive information, the protein-synthesis-dependent process is achieved once more ( Figure 11.3e ). 53

These results were partially replicated in a widely studied hippocampus-dependent memory task, the Morris water maze (WM). In this task, animals escape from cool water by finding a hidden platform underwater. To do so, animals learn spatial cues around the room to locate the platform. 54 Rats were trained for either 3 or 5 consecutive days in the WM task. Seven days later on the memory reactivation session, rats swam for 60 sec without the platform and memory was assessed by counting the number of crossings to the platform location during training.

Clearly, the animals trained for 5 days performed much better than those trained only for 3 days. Thus, 3-day trained rats were designated middle-trained and 5-day trained rats were referred to as well-trained. When tested 7 days after the reactivation session, middle-trained subjects infused with a consolidation blocker in the dorsal hippocampus on reactivation performed poorly compared to the corresponding vehicle group. However, the same treatment did not affect consolidated memory in well-trained animals, presumably because no further updating was attained. 55 Similarly, Morris et al. 56 reported that asymptotic WM task performance was not affected by protein synthesis inhibition in the dorsal hippocampus.

Conversely, task performance was disrupted by the same treatment when updating information was continuously acquired. They trained rats for 6 days in the WM task with the platform in a constant position. On day 7, retrieval was accounted by a single trial and anisomycin was immediately injected locally. Under these conditions memory was unimpaired. Interestingly, when the platform location was changed daily during training, anisomycin injection after retrieval on day 7 disrupted previously consolidated memory. The authors concluded that acquisition of new information is required to observe consolidated memory susceptibility to protein synthesis inhibition.

Thus, the so-called reconsolidation seems more like an updating consolidation intended to modify retrieved memory by a process that integrates updated experience into long-term memory. Previously consolidated memory is partially destabilized and by the infusion of disrupting agents it appears as if the process is intended to consolidate memory again. Two important features must be stressed about the updating consolidation process: it is time- and protein-synthesis-dependent. These features again bring attention to the cellular changes that account for LTM, i.e., the stabilization of neural ensembles. 4

Updating consolidation may be the process by means of which neural ensembles are modified and stabilized into updated memory traces. This proposal is based on the analysis of behavior, and even though it is clear that behavior is not merely a reflection of memory; we think it is possible to outline some of the changes that the updating consolidation process may produce in the memory traces based on the behavioral observations depicted above (for more on the behavior–memory dichotomy see Chapter 1 ). In a simple scenario, two types of information can modify behavior.

Reinforcement of previous learning

As with a learning curve, previous learning is strengthened on each trial because information of the same quality is acquired. Keeping in mind that a neuronal ensemble underlies a particular memory, reinforcing information may modify the existing consolidated trace by two means: by making the synaptic weights of the already existing ensemble stronger or by addition of cellular entities to the previously consolidated ensemble. In both cases, modifications of the synaptic weights involved in the ensemble are required.

Using artificial neural network simulation, it has been proposed that in order to preserve old memories while learning sequential new patterns, an active maintenance process is required. Otherwise, old memory is lost with incoming information. In this model, new learning is incorporated to old patterns by partially rehearsing the old ones. Importantly, modifications in synaptic weights are needed if the ensemble is to retain previously stored material while learning new information. 57

Updating consolidation is the proposed mechanism that permits modifications of the ensemble. By protein synthesis inhibition, updating consolidation is unveiled in the limited disruption of previously acquired information that is less noticeable as plateau performance is reached. Consistently, lack of memory disruption by protein synthesis inhibitors correlates with the asymptotical level in task performance. In this regard, positive modulation of a retrieved memory was reported in crabs. 58 The study showed that through retrieval, a weak memory is strengthened by an endogenous brain mechanism mediated by angiotensin II. Although, positive modulation of retrieved memories has been reported, 14 , 59 this study was done to shed light upon the functional value of reconsolidation. In accordance with the updating consolidation proposal, the researchers concluded that reconsolidation is a state for modifying memory strength.

A last piece of evidence comes from memory studies in chicks. Summers et al. 60 reported that a weak memory is strengthened by means of retrieval. They suggested that memory retrieval initiates a mechanism that allows incorporation of information acquired in the retrieval session to LTM. However, memory retrieval was found to modify memory as long as consolidation was not accomplished. Thus, this mechanism in chicks seems limited to the time before memory is stored. 60 , 61 This is not the case in rats, where memory can be modified after consolidation is attained and even more, as noted above, limited disruption of previously consolidated memory is observed.

Shift of previous learning

Divergent information is integrated to previously consolidated memory. For example, taste aversion can be learned from a taste already tagged as a safe stimulus. 53 , 62 Under these conditions, taste aversion requires a protein synthesis-dependent process to be stored in the long term. One possibility for this integration to occur is that the ensemble suffers greater modifications than when strengthening information is acquired. On the other hand, a different but overlapping trace would be created for this divergent information, in which case modifications in the synaptic weights involved in the first ensemble would take place as well. Overlapping between the ensembles is supposed because they represent divergent associations between overlapping stimuli. In our taste memory example, this overlapping is uncovered on the aversion level that animals show to a taste previously tagged as safe ( Figure 11.3e ). Less aversion is observed when compared to the aversion shown to the same taste when novel. Reinforcement of aversive learning is required to reach similar aversion levels when the taste was previously learned as safe.

Another example has been reported in the Manduca sexta moth. Daly and colleagues 63 found progressive neural recruitment and changes on network activity over the course of olfactory conditioning. Their results point to the idea that olfactory memory traces are modified upon experience. Hence, memories are not separate entities; rather they overlap to some extent. Overlapping of the traces is determined by the similarities of the involved information. This idea is in line with previous theories regarding incorporation of recent experiences into long-term knowledge background. 64–66

Extinction is another possibility in which a memory trace can be updated by divergent information. Ample evidence indicates that extinction is not forgetting nor erasing of conditioning learning, but a related learning that elicits a behavioral shift upon CS presentation. 67 This evidence is congruent with the proposed model of updating consolidation. Updating does not imply erasing of previous learning but incorporation of a related learning that takes over behavior. 53

Eisenberg and Dudai 38 consistently reported that disrupted memory upon retrieval is recovered by presentation of an unpaired reinforcer. Similarly, contextual conditioning memory disrupted after retrieval was shown to recover spontaneously after 21 days. 68 Power et al. 69 found contextual-conditioning memory recovery 6 days after acquisition and, moreover; using the same memory task, Prado-Alcalá et al. 70 reported that repeated retrieval sessions are sufficient to fully recover memory previously affected on retrieval by tetrodotoxin injections.

These results suggest that disruption of the reconsolidation process does not abolish consolidated memory; instead, the observed memory destabilization known as reconsolidation may be related to interference with the proposed updating process aimed at the integration of related learning. The result of this process will be stable overlapping traces. The relationships of these converging (trace reinforcement) or diverging (trace shift) overlapping traces will determine behavior.

Recently, it was reported that reconsolidation and integration of new information to memory are dissociable processes. Animals were conditioned using a light and a context as CSs, and footshocks as USs. On retrieval, a new context and the same light were presented without the US. As a consequence, an association was established between the first conditioning, i.e, the first CS-US association, and the new context. When tested, animals elicited the conditioned response (freezing) when placed in the second context, that is, a new CS.

The association between a CS and a previously acquired CS-US association is called second order conditioning. Inhibition of the transcription factor C/EBPβ in the hippocampus disrupted LTM of the second conditioning. Conversely, if retrieval is assessed presenting the same stimuli as in acquisition C/EBPβ is required in the amygdala for memory to remain. Thus, it was concluded that linking new of information occurs without destabilizing the retrieved memory. 71

Contrary to this, we found in two different memory tasks and regions that partial disruption of consolidated memory is observed by consolidation blockers upon memory updating. Moreover, protein synthesis is still required in the same region even though updated information is of different quality. It is important to note that the differences in molecular mechanisms found by Tronel et al. 71 indeed support the view that linking new information is not the same as reinforcement of previous learning (as reconsolidation is not a bona fide copy of consolidation), but do not support that reconsolidation is not intended to update memory.

The important issue is that reinforcement of previous learning is not synonymous with reconsolidation and that linking new information is different from a reconsolidation process. On their retrieval protocol, Tronel et al. used either the same (which they called reconsolidation) or different information (another context they called linking new information) compared to acquisition. Integration of information of one kind does not necessarily imply that integration of another kind requires the same mechanisms. All in all, we consider that evidence is accumulating for the hypothesis that reconsolidation is indeed an updating consolidation. In this time- and protein-synthesis-dependent process, retrieved memory seems to be modified by the integration of updated relevant experience.

  • ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Oreste Carbajal for technical assistance. Part of the work described in this chapter was supported by CONACYT-México 42657/A-1 and DGAPA.-UNAM IN-220706-3.

Process by which a cell converts an extracellular signal into a response. 5

Synthesis of RNA on DNA template. 6

Synthesis of protein on mRNA template. 6

In this protocol, a context (CS) like a particular chamber is associated with a footshock (US). As with fear conditioning, the response used as a measure of memory is freezing (in this case, a reaction to the chamber, not to a tone).

This kind of memory reflects the judgment of previous experience with particular stimuli. The tasks commonly rely on the natural tendency of rodents to explore new stimuli. In the first phase, animals are habituated to a novel stimulus like a light bulb. After a delay, the second phase involves presentation of a copy of the bulb along with some other stimulus like a glass jar. During this phase, the animals explore the jar over the bulb, indicating that the jar is a new stimulus and the bulb a familiar one, that is, the bulb is recognized as a familiar stimulus.

  • Cite this Page Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Memory Reconsolidation or Updating Consolidation? In: Bermúdez-Rattoni F, editor. Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes to Brain Imaging. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007. Chapter 11.

In this Page

  • INTRODUCTION
  • CONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS
  • RECONSOLIDATION ERA
  • ON RESTRAINTS OF RECONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS
  • CONSOLIDATION AND RECONSOLIDATION: THE SAME PROCESS?
  • RECONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS RECONSIDERED: UPDATING CONSOLIDATION PROPOSAL

Other titles in this collection

  • Frontiers in Neuroscience

Related information

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Similar articles in PubMed

  • Review Determinants to trigger memory reconsolidation: The role of retrieval and updating information. [Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017] Review Determinants to trigger memory reconsolidation: The role of retrieval and updating information. Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017 Jul; 142(Pt A):4-12. Epub 2016 Dec 21.
  • Intrahippocampal anisomycin infusions disrupt previously consolidated spatial memory only when memory is updated. [Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008] Intrahippocampal anisomycin infusions disrupt previously consolidated spatial memory only when memory is updated. Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Garcia-DeLaTorre P, Benavidez E, Ballesteros MA, Bermudez-Rattoni F. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008 Mar; 89(3):352-9. Epub 2007 Nov 28.
  • Reconsolidation may incorporate state-dependency into previously consolidated memories. [Learn Mem. 2013] Reconsolidation may incorporate state-dependency into previously consolidated memories. Sierra RO, Cassini LF, Santana F, Crestani AP, Duran JM, Haubrich J, de Oliveira Alvares L, Quillfeldt JA. Learn Mem. 2013 Jun 19; 20(7):379-87. Epub 2013 Jun 19.
  • Reconsolidation of fresh, remote, and extinguished fear memory in Medaka: old fears don't die. [Eur J Neurosci. 2004] Reconsolidation of fresh, remote, and extinguished fear memory in Medaka: old fears don't die. Eisenberg M, Dudai Y. Eur J Neurosci. 2004 Dec; 20(12):3397-403.
  • Review Molecular Mechanisms of Reconsolidation-Dependent Memory Updating. [Int J Mol Sci. 2020] Review Molecular Mechanisms of Reconsolidation-Dependent Memory Updating. Bellfy L, Kwapis JL. Int J Mol Sci. 2020 Sep 9; 21(18). Epub 2020 Sep 9.

Recent Activity

  • Memory Reconsolidation or Updating Consolidation? - Neural Plasticity and Memory Memory Reconsolidation or Updating Consolidation? - Neural Plasticity and Memory

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Consolidation

Consolidation refers to the process by which memories are strengthened and stabilized in the brain. It involves transferring information from short-term memory to long-term memory storage.

" Consolidation " appears in:

Subjects ( 1 ).

  • AP European History

Practice Questions ( 5 )

  • Which of the following experiments would best test the hypothesis that sleep deprivation disrupts the consolidation of long-term memories?
  • How does sleep affect consolidation of declarative memories?
  • Which of the following best exemplifies the process of consolidation in Neuropsychology?
  • What experiment would best examine the impact of sleep deprivation on the consolidation of episodic memories?
  • Which region would be targeted by an intervention aiming to aid consolidation from short-term to long-term memory for someone with anterograde amnesia?

Related terms

Encoding : The initial process of acquiring information and transforming it into a format that can be stored in memory.

Retrieval : The process of accessing and recalling stored information from memory.

Hippocampus : A brain structure involved in the formation and consolidation of new memories.

Fiveable

Stay Connected

© 2024 Fiveable Inc. All rights reserved.

AP® and SAT® are trademarks registered by the College Board, which is not affiliated with, and does not endorse this website.

COMMENTS

  1. Memory consolidation

    Memory consolidation is a category of processes that stabilize a memory trace after its initial acquisition. [1] A memory trace is a change in the nervous system caused by memorizing something. Consolidation is distinguished into two specific processes. The first, synaptic consolidation, which is thought to correspond to late-phase long-term ...

  2. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    the hypothesis that information passes through two stages in memory formation. During the first stage, the memory is held by perseveration (repetition) of neural activity and is easily disrupted. During the second stage, the memory becomes fixed, or consolidated, and is no longer easily disrupted. The perseveration-consolidation hypothesis ...

  3. Consolidation and reconsolidation: Two lives of memories?

    Consolidation as linking cortical representations . There are several variations of the hypothesis that the hippocampus rapidly stores critical information for linking cortical representations and that during multiple iterations of cortical-hippocampal interaction, connections within the cortex are strengthened and eventually support these associations in the absence of hippocampal function ...

  4. What Is Memory Consolidation?

    Memory consolidation relies on synapses in the brain. In something akin to an electrical system conducting a current,r the synapses pass signals from neuron to neuron with the help of neurotransmitters. The more frequently signals are passed, the stronger the synapses become. This process, called potentiation, is believed to play a major role ...

  5. 12 Two Distinct Stages of Memory Consolidation

    The consolidation hypothesis received little attention in the twentieth century until the later 1940s, when studies showed that electrical currents applied to the head sufficient to produce overt convulsions (electroconvulsive shock; used successfully to treat psychiatric disorders such as major depression) produced a retrograde amnesia.

  6. Memory Consolidation

    Systems consolidation is typically, and accurately, described as the process by which memories, initially dependent on the hippocampus, are reorganized as time passes. By this process, the hippocampus gradually becomes less important for storage and retrieval, and a more permanent memory develops in distributed regions of the neocortex.

  7. Memory Consolidation

    Memory consolidation is a fundamental process of long-term memory formation, as, in fact, has been described to occur in a multitude of different types of memories, species, and memory systems. It refers to the stabilization process of a newly formed long-term memory. Initially, the memory is in a fragile state and can be disrupted by several ...

  8. Memory--a century of consolidation

    Abstract. The memory consolidation hypothesis proposed 100 years ago by Müller and Pilzecker continues to guide memory research. The hypothesis that new memories consolidate slowly over time has stimulated studies revealing the hormonal and neural influences regulating memory consolidation, as well as molecular and cellular mechanisms.

  9. Memory Consolidation

    The view of consolidation advanced by the pioneering experimental psychologists Müller and Pilzecker was not embraced by the field of experimental psychology in the latter half of the twentieth century. Ironically, during that same period of time, the notion that memories consolidate became the "standard story" in the field of neuroscience.

  10. 100 Years of Consolidation— Remembering Müller and Pilzecker

    The origin of the concept of memory consolidation and the introduction of the term "consolidirung" (consolidation) to the modern science of memory are generally credited to Georg Elias Müller (1850-1934), professor at the University of Göttingen, Germany, and his student Alfons Pilzecker. Their seminal monograph "Experimentelle ...

  11. Memory consolidation.

    This chapter introduces the concept of multiple memory systems, which brain structures are engaged, and how they interact through the process of consolidation. Memory consolidation takes place both at the microscopic level, in the form of changes in synaptic strength, and at the whole-brain systems level, where it refers to a representational shift between different neural networks. We focus ...

  12. Reconsolidation and the Dynamic Nature of Memory

    Reconsolidation, as we discussed above, has been defined by applying the very standards that define consolidation. Therefore, certain nonspecific interpretations of the reconsolidation hypothesis pose the same challenges to the consolidation hypothesis, a consequence that is rarely acknowledged.

  13. PERSEVERATION-CONSOLIDATION HYPOTHESIS

    the hypothesis postulating that data goes through two phases in memory cultivation. During the first phase, the memory is retained by perseveration of neural activity and is simply disturbed. During the second phase, the memory comes to be fixed, or integrated, and is no longer simply disturbed.

  14. A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation

    As a consequence of the perceived inability of the consolidation hypothesis to account for reconsolidation, new memory models were developed that treated new and reactivated consolidated memories ...

  15. Mechanisms of Memory Consolidation and Transformation

    One proposed mechanism is the 'active consolidation in sleep' hypothesis. ... but it is clear from the dictionary definition of the term consolidation, 'to make firm', that it is a misnomer. Cellular consolidation, systems consolidation, and reconsolidation are all part of a dynamic, non-linear process. ... Department of Psychology and ...

  16. Memory Consolidation: Definition And Examples In Psychology

    Also called an engram, a memory trace is a change in the nervous system that is thought to encode a memory. These memory traces assist us in reconstructing memories. After the memory trace is created, consolidation strengthens it through changes in neural pathways. The exact alterations that occur to create a memory trace are unknown; but ...

  17. Memory Consolidation

    Memory consolidation in psychology is how the brain transforms short-term memories into long-term memories. The main characteristics of the memory consolidation process include the following:

  18. Memory Consolidation

    Memory consolidation is a fundamental process of long-term memory formation, as, in fact, has been described to occur in a multitude of different types of memories, species, and memory systems. It refers to the stabilization process of a newly formed long-term memory. Initially, the memory is in a fragile state and can be disrupted by several ...

  19. Memory integration: An alternative to the consolidation/reconsolidation

    The theoretical hypothesis is derived from the idea that newly formed memories are initially labile and sensitive to disruption, but over time undergo a time-dependent 'consolidation' process that converts them into a stable form largely resistant to disruption (Fig. 2).As a consequence, the consolidation hypothesis posited that, once memory was consolidated into storage, amnesia could not ...

  20. Memory Reconsolidation or Updating Consolidation?

    For a long time, consolidation was seen as a process achieved only on newly acquired memories aimed to store them for the long term. However, pioneer and recent studies have demonstrated that after retrieval, long-term memories may once more undergo a consolidation-like process referred to as reconsolidation. Mainly, reconsolidation is sustained by the now widely reported observation that ...

  21. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    A trusted reference in the field of psychology, offering more than 25,000 clear and authoritative entries. ... consolidation. Share button. Updated on 04/19/2018. ... See perseveration-consolidation hypothesis. Browse Dictionary.

  22. Systems Consolidation

    Learning and memory. In Fundamentals of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2013. 3.3 System consolidation: interaction between the medial temporal lobes and the neocortex. System consolidation can take much longer to complete and may range from days to years or decades. Patients with MTL lesions show a retrograde memory loss that is temporally graded, so recent memory loss (before the amnesia) is greater ...

  23. Consolidation

    AP Psychology; Consolidation; Consolidation. Definition. Consolidation refers to the process by which memories are strengthened and stabilized in the brain. It involves transferring information from short-term memory to long-term memory storage. ... Practice Questions (5) Which of the following experiments would best test the hypothesis that ...