Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base

Methodology

  • Guide to Experimental Design | Overview, Steps, & Examples

Guide to Experimental Design | Overview, 5 steps & Examples

Published on December 3, 2019 by Rebecca Bevans . Revised on June 21, 2023.

Experiments are used to study causal relationships . You manipulate one or more independent variables and measure their effect on one or more dependent variables.

Experimental design create a set of procedures to systematically test a hypothesis . A good experimental design requires a strong understanding of the system you are studying.

There are five key steps in designing an experiment:

  • Consider your variables and how they are related
  • Write a specific, testable hypothesis
  • Design experimental treatments to manipulate your independent variable
  • Assign subjects to groups, either between-subjects or within-subjects
  • Plan how you will measure your dependent variable

For valid conclusions, you also need to select a representative sample and control any  extraneous variables that might influence your results. If random assignment of participants to control and treatment groups is impossible, unethical, or highly difficult, consider an observational study instead. This minimizes several types of research bias, particularly sampling bias , survivorship bias , and attrition bias as time passes.

Table of contents

Step 1: define your variables, step 2: write your hypothesis, step 3: design your experimental treatments, step 4: assign your subjects to treatment groups, step 5: measure your dependent variable, other interesting articles, frequently asked questions about experiments.

You should begin with a specific research question . We will work with two research question examples, one from health sciences and one from ecology:

To translate your research question into an experimental hypothesis, you need to define the main variables and make predictions about how they are related.

Start by simply listing the independent and dependent variables .

Research question Independent variable Dependent variable
Phone use and sleep Minutes of phone use before sleep Hours of sleep per night
Temperature and soil respiration Air temperature just above the soil surface CO2 respired from soil

Then you need to think about possible extraneous and confounding variables and consider how you might control  them in your experiment.

Extraneous variable How to control
Phone use and sleep in sleep patterns among individuals. measure the average difference between sleep with phone use and sleep without phone use rather than the average amount of sleep per treatment group.
Temperature and soil respiration also affects respiration, and moisture can decrease with increasing temperature. monitor soil moisture and add water to make sure that soil moisture is consistent across all treatment plots.

Finally, you can put these variables together into a diagram. Use arrows to show the possible relationships between variables and include signs to show the expected direction of the relationships.

Diagram of the relationship between variables in a sleep experiment

Here we predict that increasing temperature will increase soil respiration and decrease soil moisture, while decreasing soil moisture will lead to decreased soil respiration.

Here's why students love Scribbr's proofreading services

Discover proofreading & editing

Now that you have a strong conceptual understanding of the system you are studying, you should be able to write a specific, testable hypothesis that addresses your research question.

Null hypothesis (H ) Alternate hypothesis (H )
Phone use and sleep Phone use before sleep does not correlate with the amount of sleep a person gets. Increasing phone use before sleep leads to a decrease in sleep.
Temperature and soil respiration Air temperature does not correlate with soil respiration. Increased air temperature leads to increased soil respiration.

The next steps will describe how to design a controlled experiment . In a controlled experiment, you must be able to:

  • Systematically and precisely manipulate the independent variable(s).
  • Precisely measure the dependent variable(s).
  • Control any potential confounding variables.

If your study system doesn’t match these criteria, there are other types of research you can use to answer your research question.

How you manipulate the independent variable can affect the experiment’s external validity – that is, the extent to which the results can be generalized and applied to the broader world.

First, you may need to decide how widely to vary your independent variable.

  • just slightly above the natural range for your study region.
  • over a wider range of temperatures to mimic future warming.
  • over an extreme range that is beyond any possible natural variation.

Second, you may need to choose how finely to vary your independent variable. Sometimes this choice is made for you by your experimental system, but often you will need to decide, and this will affect how much you can infer from your results.

  • a categorical variable : either as binary (yes/no) or as levels of a factor (no phone use, low phone use, high phone use).
  • a continuous variable (minutes of phone use measured every night).

How you apply your experimental treatments to your test subjects is crucial for obtaining valid and reliable results.

First, you need to consider the study size : how many individuals will be included in the experiment? In general, the more subjects you include, the greater your experiment’s statistical power , which determines how much confidence you can have in your results.

Then you need to randomly assign your subjects to treatment groups . Each group receives a different level of the treatment (e.g. no phone use, low phone use, high phone use).

You should also include a control group , which receives no treatment. The control group tells us what would have happened to your test subjects without any experimental intervention.

When assigning your subjects to groups, there are two main choices you need to make:

  • A completely randomized design vs a randomized block design .
  • A between-subjects design vs a within-subjects design .

Randomization

An experiment can be completely randomized or randomized within blocks (aka strata):

  • In a completely randomized design , every subject is assigned to a treatment group at random.
  • In a randomized block design (aka stratified random design), subjects are first grouped according to a characteristic they share, and then randomly assigned to treatments within those groups.
Completely randomized design Randomized block design
Phone use and sleep Subjects are all randomly assigned a level of phone use using a random number generator. Subjects are first grouped by age, and then phone use treatments are randomly assigned within these groups.
Temperature and soil respiration Warming treatments are assigned to soil plots at random by using a number generator to generate map coordinates within the study area. Soils are first grouped by average rainfall, and then treatment plots are randomly assigned within these groups.

Sometimes randomization isn’t practical or ethical , so researchers create partially-random or even non-random designs. An experimental design where treatments aren’t randomly assigned is called a quasi-experimental design .

Between-subjects vs. within-subjects

In a between-subjects design (also known as an independent measures design or classic ANOVA design), individuals receive only one of the possible levels of an experimental treatment.

In medical or social research, you might also use matched pairs within your between-subjects design to make sure that each treatment group contains the same variety of test subjects in the same proportions.

In a within-subjects design (also known as a repeated measures design), every individual receives each of the experimental treatments consecutively, and their responses to each treatment are measured.

Within-subjects or repeated measures can also refer to an experimental design where an effect emerges over time, and individual responses are measured over time in order to measure this effect as it emerges.

Counterbalancing (randomizing or reversing the order of treatments among subjects) is often used in within-subjects designs to ensure that the order of treatment application doesn’t influence the results of the experiment.

Between-subjects (independent measures) design Within-subjects (repeated measures) design
Phone use and sleep Subjects are randomly assigned a level of phone use (none, low, or high) and follow that level of phone use throughout the experiment. Subjects are assigned consecutively to zero, low, and high levels of phone use throughout the experiment, and the order in which they follow these treatments is randomized.
Temperature and soil respiration Warming treatments are assigned to soil plots at random and the soils are kept at this temperature throughout the experiment. Every plot receives each warming treatment (1, 3, 5, 8, and 10C above ambient temperatures) consecutively over the course of the experiment, and the order in which they receive these treatments is randomized.

Prevent plagiarism. Run a free check.

Finally, you need to decide how you’ll collect data on your dependent variable outcomes. You should aim for reliable and valid measurements that minimize research bias or error.

Some variables, like temperature, can be objectively measured with scientific instruments. Others may need to be operationalized to turn them into measurable observations.

  • Ask participants to record what time they go to sleep and get up each day.
  • Ask participants to wear a sleep tracker.

How precisely you measure your dependent variable also affects the kinds of statistical analysis you can use on your data.

Experiments are always context-dependent, and a good experimental design will take into account all of the unique considerations of your study system to produce information that is both valid and relevant to your research question.

If you want to know more about statistics , methodology , or research bias , make sure to check out some of our other articles with explanations and examples.

  • Student’s  t -distribution
  • Normal distribution
  • Null and Alternative Hypotheses
  • Chi square tests
  • Confidence interval
  • Cluster sampling
  • Stratified sampling
  • Data cleansing
  • Reproducibility vs Replicability
  • Peer review
  • Likert scale

Research bias

  • Implicit bias
  • Framing effect
  • Cognitive bias
  • Placebo effect
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Hindsight bias
  • Affect heuristic

Experimental design means planning a set of procedures to investigate a relationship between variables . To design a controlled experiment, you need:

  • A testable hypothesis
  • At least one independent variable that can be precisely manipulated
  • At least one dependent variable that can be precisely measured

When designing the experiment, you decide:

  • How you will manipulate the variable(s)
  • How you will control for any potential confounding variables
  • How many subjects or samples will be included in the study
  • How subjects will be assigned to treatment levels

Experimental design is essential to the internal and external validity of your experiment.

The key difference between observational studies and experimental designs is that a well-done observational study does not influence the responses of participants, while experiments do have some sort of treatment condition applied to at least some participants by random assignment .

A confounding variable , also called a confounder or confounding factor, is a third variable in a study examining a potential cause-and-effect relationship.

A confounding variable is related to both the supposed cause and the supposed effect of the study. It can be difficult to separate the true effect of the independent variable from the effect of the confounding variable.

In your research design , it’s important to identify potential confounding variables and plan how you will reduce their impact.

In a between-subjects design , every participant experiences only one condition, and researchers assess group differences between participants in various conditions.

In a within-subjects design , each participant experiences all conditions, and researchers test the same participants repeatedly for differences between conditions.

The word “between” means that you’re comparing different conditions between groups, while the word “within” means you’re comparing different conditions within the same group.

An experimental group, also known as a treatment group, receives the treatment whose effect researchers wish to study, whereas a control group does not. They should be identical in all other ways.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.

Bevans, R. (2023, June 21). Guide to Experimental Design | Overview, 5 steps & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved August 29, 2024, from https://www.scribbr.com/methodology/experimental-design/

Is this article helpful?

Rebecca Bevans

Rebecca Bevans

Other students also liked, random assignment in experiments | introduction & examples, quasi-experimental design | definition, types & examples, how to write a lab report, "i thought ai proofreading was useless but..".

I've been using Scribbr for years now and I know it's a service that won't disappoint. It does a good job spotting mistakes”

  • Info Videos
  • What’s New in PASS 2024?
  • PASS Documentation
  • System Requirements
  • Publications Citing PASS
  • Customer Satisfaction
  • Plot Capabilities
  • What’s New in NCSS 2024?
  • NCSS Documentation
  • Academic Institutions
  • Publications Citing NCSS
  • PASS 2024 (Sample Size)
  • NCSS 2024 (Data Analysis)
  • Medical Research
  • Business Research
  • Quality Control
  • Mass Appraisal
  • PASS Videos
  • PASS Training Videos
  • PASS Downloads & Updates
  • PASS System Requirements
  • PASS License Agreements
  • NCSS Videos
  • NCSS Training Videos
  • NCSS Downloads & Updates
  • NCSS System Requirements
  • NCSS License Agreements
  • Online Store
  • Student Store
  • Custom Payment
  • Price Lists
  • Documentation

Design of Experiments in NCSS

Introduction, technical details, randomization lists, balanced incomplete block designs, fractional factorial designs, latin square designs, response surface designs, screening designs, taguchi designs, two-level designs, design generator, d-optimal designs, procedure input.

Randomization Lists - Procedure Window

Sample Output

Randomization Lists - Sample Output

1 A B C
2 A B D
3 A C D
4 B C D
  • 1. Randomly assign the numbers to the blocks.
  • 2. Randomly assign the letters to the treatments.
  • 3. Randomly assign the treatments within the blocks.
  • 4. Randomly group blocks as replicates. A replicate is a complete set of all treatments.

Balanced Incomplete Block Designs - Sample Output

A B C D
B C D A
C D A B
D A B C
Aa Bb Cc Dd
Bd Ca Db Ac
Cb Dc Ad Ba
Dc Ad Ba Cb

Latin Square Designs - Sample Output

  • 1. The low-level value is assigned to -1.
  • 2. The high-level value is assigned to 1.
  • 3. The average of these two values is assigned to 0.
  • 4. The values of - a and a are used to find the minimum and the maximum values.
45
50
55
60
65
1.41
1.73
2.00
2.00
2.24

Response Surface Designs - Sample Output

Start Your Free 30 Day Trial Now Buy Now

"PASS 13 is fantastic! Better than my new dishwasher and microwave combined."

— Dr. Barbara Tabachnick , CSUN, Author

"Your [PASS] routines are flexible and easy to use and your customer service and support is A-double-plus."

— Nayak Polissar , PhD

Sample Size

  • What’s New in PASS 2024?

Data Analysis

Free Trials

Copyright © 2024 NCSS. All trademarks are the properties of their respective owners. Privacy Policy | Terms of Use | Sitemap

NCSS

  • Privacy Policy

Research Method

Home » Experimental Design – Types, Methods, Guide

Experimental Design – Types, Methods, Guide

Table of Contents

Experimental Research Design

Experimental Design

Experimental design is a process of planning and conducting scientific experiments to investigate a hypothesis or research question. It involves carefully designing an experiment that can test the hypothesis, and controlling for other variables that may influence the results.

Experimental design typically includes identifying the variables that will be manipulated or measured, defining the sample or population to be studied, selecting an appropriate method of sampling, choosing a method for data collection and analysis, and determining the appropriate statistical tests to use.

Types of Experimental Design

Here are the different types of experimental design:

Completely Randomized Design

In this design, participants are randomly assigned to one of two or more groups, and each group is exposed to a different treatment or condition.

Randomized Block Design

This design involves dividing participants into blocks based on a specific characteristic, such as age or gender, and then randomly assigning participants within each block to one of two or more treatment groups.

Factorial Design

In a factorial design, participants are randomly assigned to one of several groups, each of which receives a different combination of two or more independent variables.

Repeated Measures Design

In this design, each participant is exposed to all of the different treatments or conditions, either in a random order or in a predetermined order.

Crossover Design

This design involves randomly assigning participants to one of two or more treatment groups, with each group receiving one treatment during the first phase of the study and then switching to a different treatment during the second phase.

Split-plot Design

In this design, the researcher manipulates one or more variables at different levels and uses a randomized block design to control for other variables.

Nested Design

This design involves grouping participants within larger units, such as schools or households, and then randomly assigning these units to different treatment groups.

Laboratory Experiment

Laboratory experiments are conducted under controlled conditions, which allows for greater precision and accuracy. However, because laboratory conditions are not always representative of real-world conditions, the results of these experiments may not be generalizable to the population at large.

Field Experiment

Field experiments are conducted in naturalistic settings and allow for more realistic observations. However, because field experiments are not as controlled as laboratory experiments, they may be subject to more sources of error.

Experimental Design Methods

Experimental design methods refer to the techniques and procedures used to design and conduct experiments in scientific research. Here are some common experimental design methods:

Randomization

This involves randomly assigning participants to different groups or treatments to ensure that any observed differences between groups are due to the treatment and not to other factors.

Control Group

The use of a control group is an important experimental design method that involves having a group of participants that do not receive the treatment or intervention being studied. The control group is used as a baseline to compare the effects of the treatment group.

Blinding involves keeping participants, researchers, or both unaware of which treatment group participants are in, in order to reduce the risk of bias in the results.

Counterbalancing

This involves systematically varying the order in which participants receive treatments or interventions in order to control for order effects.

Replication

Replication involves conducting the same experiment with different samples or under different conditions to increase the reliability and validity of the results.

This experimental design method involves manipulating multiple independent variables simultaneously to investigate their combined effects on the dependent variable.

This involves dividing participants into subgroups or blocks based on specific characteristics, such as age or gender, in order to reduce the risk of confounding variables.

Data Collection Method

Experimental design data collection methods are techniques and procedures used to collect data in experimental research. Here are some common experimental design data collection methods:

Direct Observation

This method involves observing and recording the behavior or phenomenon of interest in real time. It may involve the use of structured or unstructured observation, and may be conducted in a laboratory or naturalistic setting.

Self-report Measures

Self-report measures involve asking participants to report their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors using questionnaires, surveys, or interviews. These measures may be administered in person or online.

Behavioral Measures

Behavioral measures involve measuring participants’ behavior directly, such as through reaction time tasks or performance tests. These measures may be administered using specialized equipment or software.

Physiological Measures

Physiological measures involve measuring participants’ physiological responses, such as heart rate, blood pressure, or brain activity, using specialized equipment. These measures may be invasive or non-invasive, and may be administered in a laboratory or clinical setting.

Archival Data

Archival data involves using existing records or data, such as medical records, administrative records, or historical documents, as a source of information. These data may be collected from public or private sources.

Computerized Measures

Computerized measures involve using software or computer programs to collect data on participants’ behavior or responses. These measures may include reaction time tasks, cognitive tests, or other types of computer-based assessments.

Video Recording

Video recording involves recording participants’ behavior or interactions using cameras or other recording equipment. This method can be used to capture detailed information about participants’ behavior or to analyze social interactions.

Data Analysis Method

Experimental design data analysis methods refer to the statistical techniques and procedures used to analyze data collected in experimental research. Here are some common experimental design data analysis methods:

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics are used to summarize and describe the data collected in the study. This includes measures such as mean, median, mode, range, and standard deviation.

Inferential Statistics

Inferential statistics are used to make inferences or generalizations about a larger population based on the data collected in the study. This includes hypothesis testing and estimation.

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

ANOVA is a statistical technique used to compare means across two or more groups in order to determine whether there are significant differences between the groups. There are several types of ANOVA, including one-way ANOVA, two-way ANOVA, and repeated measures ANOVA.

Regression Analysis

Regression analysis is used to model the relationship between two or more variables in order to determine the strength and direction of the relationship. There are several types of regression analysis, including linear regression, logistic regression, and multiple regression.

Factor Analysis

Factor analysis is used to identify underlying factors or dimensions in a set of variables. This can be used to reduce the complexity of the data and identify patterns in the data.

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)

SEM is a statistical technique used to model complex relationships between variables. It can be used to test complex theories and models of causality.

Cluster Analysis

Cluster analysis is used to group similar cases or observations together based on similarities or differences in their characteristics.

Time Series Analysis

Time series analysis is used to analyze data collected over time in order to identify trends, patterns, or changes in the data.

Multilevel Modeling

Multilevel modeling is used to analyze data that is nested within multiple levels, such as students nested within schools or employees nested within companies.

Applications of Experimental Design 

Experimental design is a versatile research methodology that can be applied in many fields. Here are some applications of experimental design:

  • Medical Research: Experimental design is commonly used to test new treatments or medications for various medical conditions. This includes clinical trials to evaluate the safety and effectiveness of new drugs or medical devices.
  • Agriculture : Experimental design is used to test new crop varieties, fertilizers, and other agricultural practices. This includes randomized field trials to evaluate the effects of different treatments on crop yield, quality, and pest resistance.
  • Environmental science: Experimental design is used to study the effects of environmental factors, such as pollution or climate change, on ecosystems and wildlife. This includes controlled experiments to study the effects of pollutants on plant growth or animal behavior.
  • Psychology : Experimental design is used to study human behavior and cognitive processes. This includes experiments to test the effects of different interventions, such as therapy or medication, on mental health outcomes.
  • Engineering : Experimental design is used to test new materials, designs, and manufacturing processes in engineering applications. This includes laboratory experiments to test the strength and durability of new materials, or field experiments to test the performance of new technologies.
  • Education : Experimental design is used to evaluate the effectiveness of teaching methods, educational interventions, and programs. This includes randomized controlled trials to compare different teaching methods or evaluate the impact of educational programs on student outcomes.
  • Marketing : Experimental design is used to test the effectiveness of marketing campaigns, pricing strategies, and product designs. This includes experiments to test the impact of different marketing messages or pricing schemes on consumer behavior.

Examples of Experimental Design 

Here are some examples of experimental design in different fields:

  • Example in Medical research : A study that investigates the effectiveness of a new drug treatment for a particular condition. Patients are randomly assigned to either a treatment group or a control group, with the treatment group receiving the new drug and the control group receiving a placebo. The outcomes, such as improvement in symptoms or side effects, are measured and compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Education research: A study that examines the impact of a new teaching method on student learning outcomes. Students are randomly assigned to either a group that receives the new teaching method or a group that receives the traditional teaching method. Student achievement is measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Environmental science: A study that tests the effectiveness of a new method for reducing pollution in a river. Two sections of the river are selected, with one section treated with the new method and the other section left untreated. The water quality is measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two sections.
  • Example in Marketing research: A study that investigates the impact of a new advertising campaign on consumer behavior. Participants are randomly assigned to either a group that is exposed to the new campaign or a group that is not. Their behavior, such as purchasing or product awareness, is measured and compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Social psychology: A study that examines the effect of a new social intervention on reducing prejudice towards a marginalized group. Participants are randomly assigned to either a group that receives the intervention or a control group that does not. Their attitudes and behavior towards the marginalized group are measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two groups.

When to use Experimental Research Design 

Experimental research design should be used when a researcher wants to establish a cause-and-effect relationship between variables. It is particularly useful when studying the impact of an intervention or treatment on a particular outcome.

Here are some situations where experimental research design may be appropriate:

  • When studying the effects of a new drug or medical treatment: Experimental research design is commonly used in medical research to test the effectiveness and safety of new drugs or medical treatments. By randomly assigning patients to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the treatment is effective in improving health outcomes.
  • When evaluating the effectiveness of an educational intervention: An experimental research design can be used to evaluate the impact of a new teaching method or educational program on student learning outcomes. By randomly assigning students to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the intervention is effective in improving academic performance.
  • When testing the effectiveness of a marketing campaign: An experimental research design can be used to test the effectiveness of different marketing messages or strategies. By randomly assigning participants to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the marketing campaign is effective in changing consumer behavior.
  • When studying the effects of an environmental intervention: Experimental research design can be used to study the impact of environmental interventions, such as pollution reduction programs or conservation efforts. By randomly assigning locations or areas to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the intervention is effective in improving environmental outcomes.
  • When testing the effects of a new technology: An experimental research design can be used to test the effectiveness and safety of new technologies or engineering designs. By randomly assigning participants or locations to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the new technology is effective in achieving its intended purpose.

How to Conduct Experimental Research

Here are the steps to conduct Experimental Research:

  • Identify a Research Question : Start by identifying a research question that you want to answer through the experiment. The question should be clear, specific, and testable.
  • Develop a Hypothesis: Based on your research question, develop a hypothesis that predicts the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. The hypothesis should be clear and testable.
  • Design the Experiment : Determine the type of experimental design you will use, such as a between-subjects design or a within-subjects design. Also, decide on the experimental conditions, such as the number of independent variables, the levels of the independent variable, and the dependent variable to be measured.
  • Select Participants: Select the participants who will take part in the experiment. They should be representative of the population you are interested in studying.
  • Randomly Assign Participants to Groups: If you are using a between-subjects design, randomly assign participants to groups to control for individual differences.
  • Conduct the Experiment : Conduct the experiment by manipulating the independent variable(s) and measuring the dependent variable(s) across the different conditions.
  • Analyze the Data: Analyze the data using appropriate statistical methods to determine if there is a significant effect of the independent variable(s) on the dependent variable(s).
  • Draw Conclusions: Based on the data analysis, draw conclusions about the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. If the results support the hypothesis, then it is accepted. If the results do not support the hypothesis, then it is rejected.
  • Communicate the Results: Finally, communicate the results of the experiment through a research report or presentation. Include the purpose of the study, the methods used, the results obtained, and the conclusions drawn.

Purpose of Experimental Design 

The purpose of experimental design is to control and manipulate one or more independent variables to determine their effect on a dependent variable. Experimental design allows researchers to systematically investigate causal relationships between variables, and to establish cause-and-effect relationships between the independent and dependent variables. Through experimental design, researchers can test hypotheses and make inferences about the population from which the sample was drawn.

Experimental design provides a structured approach to designing and conducting experiments, ensuring that the results are reliable and valid. By carefully controlling for extraneous variables that may affect the outcome of the study, experimental design allows researchers to isolate the effect of the independent variable(s) on the dependent variable(s), and to minimize the influence of other factors that may confound the results.

Experimental design also allows researchers to generalize their findings to the larger population from which the sample was drawn. By randomly selecting participants and using statistical techniques to analyze the data, researchers can make inferences about the larger population with a high degree of confidence.

Overall, the purpose of experimental design is to provide a rigorous, systematic, and scientific method for testing hypotheses and establishing cause-and-effect relationships between variables. Experimental design is a powerful tool for advancing scientific knowledge and informing evidence-based practice in various fields, including psychology, biology, medicine, engineering, and social sciences.

Advantages of Experimental Design 

Experimental design offers several advantages in research. Here are some of the main advantages:

  • Control over extraneous variables: Experimental design allows researchers to control for extraneous variables that may affect the outcome of the study. By manipulating the independent variable and holding all other variables constant, researchers can isolate the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable.
  • Establishing causality: Experimental design allows researchers to establish causality by manipulating the independent variable and observing its effect on the dependent variable. This allows researchers to determine whether changes in the independent variable cause changes in the dependent variable.
  • Replication : Experimental design allows researchers to replicate their experiments to ensure that the findings are consistent and reliable. Replication is important for establishing the validity and generalizability of the findings.
  • Random assignment: Experimental design often involves randomly assigning participants to conditions. This helps to ensure that individual differences between participants are evenly distributed across conditions, which increases the internal validity of the study.
  • Precision : Experimental design allows researchers to measure variables with precision, which can increase the accuracy and reliability of the data.
  • Generalizability : If the study is well-designed, experimental design can increase the generalizability of the findings. By controlling for extraneous variables and using random assignment, researchers can increase the likelihood that the findings will apply to other populations and contexts.

Limitations of Experimental Design

Experimental design has some limitations that researchers should be aware of. Here are some of the main limitations:

  • Artificiality : Experimental design often involves creating artificial situations that may not reflect real-world situations. This can limit the external validity of the findings, or the extent to which the findings can be generalized to real-world settings.
  • Ethical concerns: Some experimental designs may raise ethical concerns, particularly if they involve manipulating variables that could cause harm to participants or if they involve deception.
  • Participant bias : Participants in experimental studies may modify their behavior in response to the experiment, which can lead to participant bias.
  • Limited generalizability: The conditions of the experiment may not reflect the complexities of real-world situations. As a result, the findings may not be applicable to all populations and contexts.
  • Cost and time : Experimental design can be expensive and time-consuming, particularly if the experiment requires specialized equipment or if the sample size is large.
  • Researcher bias : Researchers may unintentionally bias the results of the experiment if they have expectations or preferences for certain outcomes.
  • Lack of feasibility : Experimental design may not be feasible in some cases, particularly if the research question involves variables that cannot be manipulated or controlled.

About the author

' src=

Muhammad Hassan

Researcher, Academic Writer, Web developer

You may also like

Phenomenology

Phenomenology – Methods, Examples and Guide

Observational Research

Observational Research – Methods and Guide

Ethnographic Research

Ethnographic Research -Types, Methods and Guide

Case Study Research

Case Study – Methods, Examples and Guide

Textual Analysis

Textual Analysis – Types, Examples and Guide

Focus Groups in Qualitative Research

Focus Groups – Steps, Examples and Guide

  • Skip to secondary menu
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

Statistics By Jim

Making statistics intuitive

Experimental Design: Definition and Types

By Jim Frost 3 Comments

What is Experimental Design?

An experimental design is a detailed plan for collecting and using data to identify causal relationships. Through careful planning, the design of experiments allows your data collection efforts to have a reasonable chance of detecting effects and testing hypotheses that answer your research questions.

An experiment is a data collection procedure that occurs in controlled conditions to identify and understand causal relationships between variables. Researchers can use many potential designs. The ultimate choice depends on their research question, resources, goals, and constraints. In some fields of study, researchers refer to experimental design as the design of experiments (DOE). Both terms are synonymous.

Scientist who developed an experimental design for her research.

Ultimately, the design of experiments helps ensure that your procedures and data will evaluate your research question effectively. Without an experimental design, you might waste your efforts in a process that, for many potential reasons, can’t answer your research question. In short, it helps you trust your results.

Learn more about Independent and Dependent Variables .

Design of Experiments: Goals & Settings

Experiments occur in many settings, ranging from psychology, social sciences, medicine, physics, engineering, and industrial and service sectors. Typically, experimental goals are to discover a previously unknown effect , confirm a known effect, or test a hypothesis.

Effects represent causal relationships between variables. For example, in a medical experiment, does the new medicine cause an improvement in health outcomes? If so, the medicine has a causal effect on the outcome.

An experimental design’s focus depends on the subject area and can include the following goals:

  • Understanding the relationships between variables.
  • Identifying the variables that have the largest impact on the outcomes.
  • Finding the input variable settings that produce an optimal result.

For example, psychologists have conducted experiments to understand how conformity affects decision-making. Sociologists have performed experiments to determine whether ethnicity affects the public reaction to staged bike thefts. These experiments map out the causal relationships between variables, and their primary goal is to understand the role of various factors.

Conversely, in a manufacturing environment, the researchers might use an experimental design to find the factors that most effectively improve their product’s strength, identify the optimal manufacturing settings, and do all that while accounting for various constraints. In short, a manufacturer’s goal is often to use experiments to improve their products cost-effectively.

In a medical experiment, the goal might be to quantify the medicine’s effect and find the optimum dosage.

Developing an Experimental Design

Developing an experimental design involves planning that maximizes the potential to collect data that is both trustworthy and able to detect causal relationships. Specifically, these studies aim to see effects when they exist in the population the researchers are studying, preferentially favor causal effects, isolate each factor’s true effect from potential confounders, and produce conclusions that you can generalize to the real world.

To accomplish these goals, experimental designs carefully manage data validity and reliability , and internal and external experimental validity. When your experiment is valid and reliable, you can expect your procedures and data to produce trustworthy results.

An excellent experimental design involves the following:

  • Lots of preplanning.
  • Developing experimental treatments.
  • Determining how to assign subjects to treatment groups.

The remainder of this article focuses on how experimental designs incorporate these essential items to accomplish their research goals.

Learn more about Data Reliability vs. Validity and Internal and External Experimental Validity .

Preplanning, Defining, and Operationalizing for Design of Experiments

A literature review is crucial for the design of experiments.

This phase of the design of experiments helps you identify critical variables, know how to measure them while ensuring reliability and validity, and understand the relationships between them. The review can also help you find ways to reduce sources of variability, which increases your ability to detect treatment effects. Notably, the literature review allows you to learn how similar studies designed their experiments and the challenges they faced.

Operationalizing a study involves taking your research question, using the background information you gathered, and formulating an actionable plan.

This process should produce a specific and testable hypothesis using data that you can reasonably collect given the resources available to the experiment.

  • Null hypothesis : The jumping exercise intervention does not affect bone density.
  • Alternative hypothesis : The jumping exercise intervention affects bone density.

To learn more about this early phase, read Five Steps for Conducting Scientific Studies with Statistical Analyses .

Formulating Treatments in Experimental Designs

In an experimental design, treatments are variables that the researchers control. They are the primary independent variables of interest. Researchers administer the treatment to the subjects or items in the experiment and want to know whether it causes changes in the outcome.

As the name implies, a treatment can be medical in nature, such as a new medicine or vaccine. But it’s a general term that applies to other things such as training programs, manufacturing settings, teaching methods, and types of fertilizers. I helped run an experiment where the treatment was a jumping exercise intervention that we hoped would increase bone density. All these treatment examples are things that potentially influence a measurable outcome.

Even when you know your treatment generally, you must carefully consider the amount. How large of a dose? If you’re comparing three different temperatures in a manufacturing process, how far apart are they? For my bone mineral density study, we had to determine how frequently the exercise sessions would occur and how long each lasted.

How you define the treatments in the design of experiments can affect your findings and the generalizability of your results.

Assigning Subjects to Experimental Groups

A crucial decision for all experimental designs is determining how researchers assign subjects to the experimental conditions—the treatment and control groups. The control group is often, but not always, the lack of a treatment. It serves as a basis for comparison by showing outcomes for subjects who don’t receive a treatment. Learn more about Control Groups .

How your experimental design assigns subjects to the groups affects how confident you can be that the findings represent true causal effects rather than mere correlation caused by confounders. Indeed, the assignment method influences how you control for confounding variables. This is the difference between correlation and causation .

Imagine a study finds that vitamin consumption correlates with better health outcomes. As a researcher, you want to be able to say that vitamin consumption causes the improvements. However, with the wrong experimental design, you might only be able to say there is an association. A confounder, and not the vitamins, might actually cause the health benefits.

Let’s explore some of the ways to assign subjects in design of experiments.

Completely Randomized Designs

A completely randomized experimental design randomly assigns all subjects to the treatment and control groups. You simply take each participant and use a random process to determine their group assignment. You can flip coins, roll a die, or use a computer. Randomized experiments must be prospective studies because they need to be able to control group assignment.

Random assignment in the design of experiments helps ensure that the groups are roughly equivalent at the beginning of the study. This equivalence at the start increases your confidence that any differences you see at the end were caused by the treatments. The randomization tends to equalize confounders between the experimental groups and, thereby, cancels out their effects, leaving only the treatment effects.

For example, in a vitamin study, the researchers can randomly assign participants to either the control or vitamin group. Because the groups are approximately equal when the experiment starts, if the health outcomes are different at the end of the study, the researchers can be confident that the vitamins caused those improvements.

Statisticians consider randomized experimental designs to be the best for identifying causal relationships.

If you can’t randomly assign subjects but want to draw causal conclusions about an intervention, consider using a quasi-experimental design .

Learn more about Randomized Controlled Trials and Random Assignment in Experiments .

Randomized Block Designs

Nuisance factors are variables that can affect the outcome, but they are not the researcher’s primary interest. Unfortunately, they can hide or distort the treatment results. When experimenters know about specific nuisance factors, they can use a randomized block design to minimize their impact.

This experimental design takes subjects with a shared “nuisance” characteristic and groups them into blocks. The participants in each block are then randomly assigned to the experimental groups. This process allows the experiment to control for known nuisance factors.

Blocking in the design of experiments reduces the impact of nuisance factors on experimental error. The analysis assesses the effects of the treatment within each block, which removes the variability between blocks. The result is that blocked experimental designs can reduce the impact of nuisance variables, increasing the ability to detect treatment effects accurately.

Suppose you’re testing various teaching methods. Because grade level likely affects educational outcomes, you might use grade level as a blocking factor. To use a randomized block design for this scenario, divide the participants by grade level and then randomly assign the members of each grade level to the experimental groups.

A standard guideline for an experimental design is to “Block what you can, randomize what you cannot.” Use blocking for a few primary nuisance factors. Then use random assignment to distribute the unblocked nuisance factors equally between the experimental conditions.

You can also use covariates to control nuisance factors. Learn about Covariates: Definition and Uses .

Observational Studies

In some experimental designs, randomly assigning subjects to the experimental conditions is impossible or unethical. The researchers simply can’t assign participants to the experimental groups. However, they can observe them in their natural groupings, measure the essential variables, and look for correlations. These observational studies are also known as quasi-experimental designs. Retrospective studies must be observational in nature because they look back at past events.

Imagine you’re studying the effects of depression on an activity. Clearly, you can’t randomly assign participants to the depression and control groups. But you can observe participants with and without depression and see how their task performance differs.

Observational studies let you perform research when you can’t control the treatment. However, quasi-experimental designs increase the problem of confounding variables. For this design of experiments, correlation does not necessarily imply causation. While special procedures can help control confounders in an observational study, you’re ultimately less confident that the results represent causal findings.

Learn more about Observational Studies .

For a good comparison, learn about the differences and tradeoffs between Observational Studies and Randomized Experiments .

Between-Subjects vs. Within-Subjects Experimental Designs

When you think of the design of experiments, you probably picture a treatment and control group. Researchers assign participants to only one of these groups, so each group contains entirely different subjects than the other groups. Analysts compare the groups at the end of the experiment. Statisticians refer to this method as a between-subjects, or independent measures, experimental design.

In a between-subjects design , you can have more than one treatment group, but each subject is exposed to only one condition, the control group or one of the treatment groups.

A potential downside to this approach is that differences between groups at the beginning can affect the results at the end. As you’ve read earlier, random assignment can reduce those differences, but it is imperfect. There will always be some variability between the groups.

In a  within-subjects experimental design , also known as repeated measures, subjects experience all treatment conditions and are measured for each. Each subject acts as their own control, which reduces variability and increases the statistical power to detect effects.

In this experimental design, you minimize pre-existing differences between the experimental conditions because they all contain the same subjects. However, the order of treatments can affect the results. Beware of practice and fatigue effects. Learn more about Repeated Measures Designs .

Assigned to one experimental condition Participates in all experimental conditions
Requires more subjects Fewer subjects
Differences between subjects in the groups can affect the results Uses same subjects in all conditions.
No order of treatment effects. Order of treatments can affect results.

Design of Experiments Examples

For example, a bone density study has three experimental groups—a control group, a stretching exercise group, and a jumping exercise group.

In a between-subjects experimental design, scientists randomly assign each participant to one of the three groups.

In a within-subjects design, all subjects experience the three conditions sequentially while the researchers measure bone density repeatedly. The procedure can switch the order of treatments for the participants to help reduce order effects.

Matched Pairs Experimental Design

A matched pairs experimental design is a between-subjects study that uses pairs of similar subjects. Researchers use this approach to reduce pre-existing differences between experimental groups. It’s yet another design of experiments method for reducing sources of variability.

Researchers identify variables likely to affect the outcome, such as demographics. When they pick a subject with a set of characteristics, they try to locate another participant with similar attributes to create a matched pair. Scientists randomly assign one member of a pair to the treatment group and the other to the control group.

On the plus side, this process creates two similar groups, and it doesn’t create treatment order effects. While matched pairs do not produce the perfectly matched groups of a within-subjects design (which uses the same subjects in all conditions), it aims to reduce variability between groups relative to a between-subjects study.

On the downside, finding matched pairs is very time-consuming. Additionally, if one member of a matched pair drops out, the other subject must leave the study too.

Learn more about Matched Pairs Design: Uses & Examples .

Another consideration is whether you’ll use a cross-sectional design (one point in time) or use a longitudinal study to track changes over time .

A case study is a research method that often serves as a precursor to a more rigorous experimental design by identifying research questions, variables, and hypotheses to test. Learn more about What is a Case Study? Definition & Examples .

In conclusion, the design of experiments is extremely sensitive to subject area concerns and the time and resources available to the researchers. Developing a suitable experimental design requires balancing a multitude of considerations. A successful design is necessary to obtain trustworthy answers to your research question and to have a reasonable chance of detecting treatment effects when they exist.

Share this:

experimental design application

Reader Interactions

' src=

March 23, 2024 at 2:35 pm

Dear Jim You wrote a superb document, I will use it in my Buistatistics course, along with your three books. Thank you very much! Miguel

' src=

March 23, 2024 at 5:43 pm

Thanks so much, Miguel! Glad this post was helpful and I trust the books will be as well.

' src=

April 10, 2023 at 4:36 am

What are the purpose and uses of experimental research design?

Comments and Questions Cancel reply

Accelerating R&D for innovation and collaboration

Desice is a cloud-based platform for researchers and engineers to plan and analyze experiments..

experimental design application

Desice is a tool for planning and analyzing your experiments. Designed to assist researchers and engineers get experimentation done right. In providing powerful tools we help you design the optimal experiment.

Starting with an efficient and optimized experimental approach, you can significantly reduce costs and shorten the time to market. It gives you the tools, models, and confidence to analyze and present data in a precise, repeatable, and verifiable method.

What our customers are saying!

We use Desice to accelerate the learning from our R&D testing. It is simple and efficient and costs fractions of the traditional equivalent software packages. Being a web app, we also benefit from new features immediately. Fully recommend it!

Very helpful tool for a fast and easy design of experiments.

Desice offers us ideal support in the creation, evaluation and interpretation of test series. This allows us to reach our goal more quickly and draw the right conclusions from our experiments.

Profile Picture CTO Again

Jeremy Lambert

Dr. hannes kitzler, dr. michael holzwarth.

Wastewater Treamtment Plant

Experimental design: Guide, steps, examples

Last updated

27 April 2023

Reviewed by

Miroslav Damyanov

Short on time? Get an AI generated summary of this article instead

Experimental research design is a scientific framework that allows you to manipulate one or more variables while controlling the test environment. 

When testing a theory or new product, it can be helpful to have a certain level of control and manipulate variables to discover different outcomes. You can use these experiments to determine cause and effect or study variable associations. 

This guide explores the types of experimental design, the steps in designing an experiment, and the advantages and limitations of experimental design. 

Make research less tedious

Dovetail streamlines research to help you uncover and share actionable insights

  • What is experimental research design?

You can determine the relationship between each of the variables by: 

Manipulating one or more independent variables (i.e., stimuli or treatments)

Applying the changes to one or more dependent variables (i.e., test groups or outcomes)

With the ability to analyze the relationship between variables and using measurable data, you can increase the accuracy of the result. 

What is a good experimental design?

A good experimental design requires: 

Significant planning to ensure control over the testing environment

Sound experimental treatments

Properly assigning subjects to treatment groups

Without proper planning, unexpected external variables can alter an experiment's outcome. 

To meet your research goals, your experimental design should include these characteristics:

Provide unbiased estimates of inputs and associated uncertainties

Enable the researcher to detect differences caused by independent variables

Include a plan for analysis and reporting of the results

Provide easily interpretable results with specific conclusions

What's the difference between experimental and quasi-experimental design?

The major difference between experimental and quasi-experimental design is the random assignment of subjects to groups. 

A true experiment relies on certain controls. Typically, the researcher designs the treatment and randomly assigns subjects to control and treatment groups. 

However, these conditions are unethical or impossible to achieve in some situations.

When it's unethical or impractical to assign participants randomly, that’s when a quasi-experimental design comes in. 

This design allows researchers to conduct a similar experiment by assigning subjects to groups based on non-random criteria. 

Another type of quasi-experimental design might occur when the researcher doesn't have control over the treatment but studies pre-existing groups after they receive different treatments.

When can a researcher conduct experimental research?

Various settings and professions can use experimental research to gather information and observe behavior in controlled settings. 

Basically, a researcher can conduct experimental research any time they want to test a theory with variable and dependent controls. 

Experimental research is an option when the project includes an independent variable and a desire to understand the relationship between cause and effect. 

  • The importance of experimental research design

Experimental research enables researchers to conduct studies that provide specific, definitive answers to questions and hypotheses. 

Researchers can test Independent variables in controlled settings to:

Test the effectiveness of a new medication

Design better products for consumers

Answer questions about human health and behavior

Developing a quality research plan means a researcher can accurately answer vital research questions with minimal error. As a result, definitive conclusions can influence the future of the independent variable. 

Types of experimental research designs

There are three main types of experimental research design. The research type you use will depend on the criteria of your experiment, your research budget, and environmental limitations. 

Pre-experimental research design

A pre-experimental research study is a basic observational study that monitors independent variables’ effects. 

During research, you observe one or more groups after applying a treatment to test whether the treatment causes any change. 

The three subtypes of pre-experimental research design are:

One-shot case study research design

This research method introduces a single test group to a single stimulus to study the results at the end of the application. 

After researchers presume the stimulus or treatment has caused changes, they gather results to determine how it affects the test subjects. 

One-group pretest-posttest design

This method uses a single test group but includes a pretest study as a benchmark. The researcher applies a test before and after the group’s exposure to a specific stimulus. 

Static group comparison design

This method includes two or more groups, enabling the researcher to use one group as a control. They apply a stimulus to one group and leave the other group static. 

A posttest study compares the results among groups. 

True experimental research design

A true experiment is the most common research method. It involves statistical analysis to prove or disprove a specific hypothesis . 

Under completely experimental conditions, researchers expose participants in two or more randomized groups to different stimuli. 

Random selection removes any potential for bias, providing more reliable results. 

These are the three main sub-groups of true experimental research design:

Posttest-only control group design

This structure requires the researcher to divide participants into two random groups. One group receives no stimuli and acts as a control while the other group experiences stimuli.

Researchers perform a test at the end of the experiment to observe the stimuli exposure results.

Pretest-posttest control group design

This test also requires two groups. It includes a pretest as a benchmark before introducing the stimulus. 

The pretest introduces multiple ways to test subjects. For instance, if the control group also experiences a change, it reveals that taking the test twice changes the results.

Solomon four-group design

This structure divides subjects into two groups, with two as control groups. Researchers assign the first control group a posttest only and the second control group a pretest and a posttest. 

The two variable groups mirror the control groups, but researchers expose them to stimuli. The ability to differentiate between groups in multiple ways provides researchers with more testing approaches for data-based conclusions. 

Quasi-experimental research design

Although closely related to a true experiment, quasi-experimental research design differs in approach and scope. 

Quasi-experimental research design doesn’t have randomly selected participants. Researchers typically divide the groups in this research by pre-existing differences. 

Quasi-experimental research is more common in educational studies, nursing, or other research projects where it's not ethical or practical to use randomized subject groups.

  • 5 steps for designing an experiment

Experimental research requires a clearly defined plan to outline the research parameters and expected goals. 

Here are five key steps in designing a successful experiment:

Step 1: Define variables and their relationship

Your experiment should begin with a question: What are you hoping to learn through your experiment? 

The relationship between variables in your study will determine your answer.

Define the independent variable (the intended stimuli) and the dependent variable (the expected effect of the stimuli). After identifying these groups, consider how you might control them in your experiment. 

Could natural variations affect your research? If so, your experiment should include a pretest and posttest. 

Step 2: Develop a specific, testable hypothesis

With a firm understanding of the system you intend to study, you can write a specific, testable hypothesis. 

What is the expected outcome of your study? 

Develop a prediction about how the independent variable will affect the dependent variable. 

How will the stimuli in your experiment affect your test subjects? 

Your hypothesis should provide a prediction of the answer to your research question . 

Step 3: Design experimental treatments to manipulate your independent variable

Depending on your experiment, your variable may be a fixed stimulus (like a medical treatment) or a variable stimulus (like a period during which an activity occurs). 

Determine which type of stimulus meets your experiment’s needs and how widely or finely to vary your stimuli. 

Step 4: Assign subjects to groups

When you have a clear idea of how to carry out your experiment, you can determine how to assemble test groups for an accurate study. 

When choosing your study groups, consider: 

The size of your experiment

Whether you can select groups randomly

Your target audience for the outcome of the study

You should be able to create groups with an equal number of subjects and include subjects that match your target audience. Remember, you should assign one group as a control and use one or more groups to study the effects of variables. 

Step 5: Plan how to measure your dependent variable

This step determines how you'll collect data to determine the study's outcome. You should seek reliable and valid measurements that minimize research bias or error. 

You can measure some data with scientific tools, while you’ll need to operationalize other forms to turn them into measurable observations.

  • Advantages of experimental research

Experimental research is an integral part of our world. It allows researchers to conduct experiments that answer specific questions. 

While researchers use many methods to conduct different experiments, experimental research offers these distinct benefits:

Researchers can determine cause and effect by manipulating variables.

It gives researchers a high level of control.

Researchers can test multiple variables within a single experiment.

All industries and fields of knowledge can use it. 

Researchers can duplicate results to promote the validity of the study .

Replicating natural settings rapidly means immediate research.

Researchers can combine it with other research methods.

It provides specific conclusions about the validity of a product, theory, or idea.

  • Disadvantages (or limitations) of experimental research

Unfortunately, no research type yields ideal conditions or perfect results. 

While experimental research might be the right choice for some studies, certain conditions could render experiments useless or even dangerous. 

Before conducting experimental research, consider these disadvantages and limitations:

Required professional qualification

Only competent professionals with an academic degree and specific training are qualified to conduct rigorous experimental research. This ensures results are unbiased and valid. 

Limited scope

Experimental research may not capture the complexity of some phenomena, such as social interactions or cultural norms. These are difficult to control in a laboratory setting.

Resource-intensive

Experimental research can be expensive, time-consuming, and require significant resources, such as specialized equipment or trained personnel.

Limited generalizability

The controlled nature means the research findings may not fully apply to real-world situations or people outside the experimental setting.

Practical or ethical concerns

Some experiments may involve manipulating variables that could harm participants or violate ethical guidelines . 

Researchers must ensure their experiments do not cause harm or discomfort to participants. 

Sometimes, recruiting a sample of people to randomly assign may be difficult. 

  • Experimental research design example

Experiments across all industries and research realms provide scientists, developers, and other researchers with definitive answers. These experiments can solve problems, create inventions, and heal illnesses. 

Product design testing is an excellent example of experimental research. 

A company in the product development phase creates multiple prototypes for testing. With a randomized selection, researchers introduce each test group to a different prototype. 

When groups experience different product designs , the company can assess which option most appeals to potential customers. 

Experimental research design provides researchers with a controlled environment to conduct experiments that evaluate cause and effect. 

Using the five steps to develop a research plan ensures you anticipate and eliminate external variables while answering life’s crucial questions.

Should you be using a customer insights hub?

Do you want to discover previous research faster?

Do you share your research findings with others?

Do you analyze research data?

Start for free today, add your research, and get to key insights faster

Editor’s picks

Last updated: 18 April 2023

Last updated: 27 February 2023

Last updated: 22 August 2024

Last updated: 5 February 2023

Last updated: 16 August 2024

Last updated: 9 March 2023

Last updated: 30 April 2024

Last updated: 12 December 2023

Last updated: 11 March 2024

Last updated: 4 July 2024

Last updated: 6 March 2024

Last updated: 5 March 2024

Last updated: 13 May 2024

Latest articles

Related topics, .css-je19u9{-webkit-align-items:flex-end;-webkit-box-align:flex-end;-ms-flex-align:flex-end;align-items:flex-end;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-flex-direction:row;-ms-flex-direction:row;flex-direction:row;-webkit-box-flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-flex-wrap:wrap;-ms-flex-wrap:wrap;flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-box-pack:center;-ms-flex-pack:center;-webkit-justify-content:center;justify-content:center;row-gap:0;text-align:center;max-width:671px;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}}@media (max-width: 799px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}} decide what to .css-1kiodld{max-height:56px;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-align-items:center;-webkit-box-align:center;-ms-flex-align:center;align-items:center;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-1kiodld{display:none;}} build next, decide what to build next.

  • Types of experimental

Log in or sign up

Get started for free

Are you suffering from a 
 lack of stability in the quality of your end product?

Design, run, and analyze experiments while collaborating in real-time with colleagues.

experimental design application

Trusted by the world's best companies across Life Sciences, Food, and Chemicals

experimental design application

Why use Effex?

Minimize experimental effort and maximizes knowledge discovery from each experiment.

Delivers the optimal experimental design tailored for your specific problem

Collaborate with other departments in our cloud-first environment

Built on OMARS (orthogonal minimally aliased response surface)

Efficient and comprehensive, captures interactions between variables; optimizes resource use

Built-in experiment management, traceability and calculations

Requires a large number of trials as complexity increases

Might not capture all possible interactions

Fails to detect interactions between variables; inefficient as only one variable changes per experiment

Manual effort to manage experiments, traceability, calculations

Time is not pre-defined: depending on the results the number of experiments can expand or contract

With DOE, you learn new things. You get information that is not in your existing data, and it paves the path to breakthroughs.

Select the best designs

Compare thousands of designs considering multiple criteria at the same time in our design catalog. That way, you can easily filter to find your ideal plans and assess the trade-off between size and quality.

Identify more effects with less experimental effort

Design experiments with more factors from the start at a lower cost than existing commercial available designs.

An ever-evolving catalog of designs

Get access to the latest design catalog, which is updated with new designs continuously.

Saves time and money

Instead of testing one thing at a time, you test multiple factors together

Finds best combo

Helps you discover the best combination of factors for the outcome you want.

Shows interactions

Reveals how different factors work together

experimental design application

Why customers use Effex.

Find the best mathematical models for you.

Proven Designs

Unique, industry-proof designs ensure optimal performance.

Cost-Effective Insights

Maximize knowledge discovery at a minimal cost.

Enhanced Collaboration

Powerful tools for better knowledge sharing and faster innovation.

Why a new platform for DoE

We help you to find the best operating conditions for your input so that your product and/or process is a success.

User-Friendly

Our platform is super easy to use with no need for software training. Just do DoE.

Flexible Payment

Pay only when you use, eliminating unused licenses.

Seamless Integration

Say goodbye to copy/paste. Data analytics has never been easier.

experimental design application

A design for every challenge

Effex helps you find the right design for your experiments.

Experiment design that is out of this world.

Meet omars..

Orthogonal minimally aliased response surface (OMARS) designs are a new mathematical matrix that lets you have easy management of a design while keeping complexity.

Used by teams across industries

Powerful, self-serve product and growth analytics to help you convert, engage, and retain more users.

Life sciences

Determine the most effective chemical components of your pharmaceuticals.

Find the most optimal proportions of each material and environmental factors.

Food and beverage

Create your recipe with the most efficient ingredients while not compromising on taste.

Discover our success

Explore Effex’s most impactfull case studies

Discover optimization and efficiency in your experiments

Whether you’re a newcomer to design of experiments or a seasoned statistician looking for a demo, we’re ready to guide you and determine if Effex is right for you.

experimental design application

Frequently asked questions

Discover our most frequently asked questions, curated for clarity and to address any questions you might have.

Effex software is special for several reasons. First, a user can easily compare experimental designs and access a large catalogue of exclusive OMARS designs to make the best decision for experimental design. Second, a user can benefit from our graphical and recommendation algorithms to select the best models. Thirdly, a user can explore competing optimal combinations of factor values to select the most appropriate one.

Of course! Records can be copied and pasted from Excel or any other spreadsheet into our platform.

In its first release, Effex allows multiple linear regression for large problems with multiple quantitative and categorical factors. This classical statistical modelling technique is combined with a novel approach to model selection and a graphical interactive interface that helps the user to discover the most influential effects.

Providing experimental design, data analysis and optimisation for multi-factor and multi-response experiments is one of the strongest features of Effex. The OMARS designs accessible through the software have already helped industries save up to 40% of experimental effort and optimise problems with up to 18 different responses.

Your designs are always available in the software's library, where you can easily share them with colleagues.

The uploaded datasets, analysis and conclusions are stored in isolated cloud storage instances on AWS. No one user has access to another user's data, and every security measure has been taken to protect this sensitive data.

Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, automatically generate references for free.

  • Knowledge Base
  • Methodology
  • A Quick Guide to Experimental Design | 5 Steps & Examples

A Quick Guide to Experimental Design | 5 Steps & Examples

Published on 11 April 2022 by Rebecca Bevans . Revised on 5 December 2022.

Experiments are used to study causal relationships . You manipulate one or more independent variables and measure their effect on one or more dependent variables.

Experimental design means creating a set of procedures to systematically test a hypothesis . A good experimental design requires a strong understanding of the system you are studying. 

There are five key steps in designing an experiment:

  • Consider your variables and how they are related
  • Write a specific, testable hypothesis
  • Design experimental treatments to manipulate your independent variable
  • Assign subjects to groups, either between-subjects or within-subjects
  • Plan how you will measure your dependent variable

For valid conclusions, you also need to select a representative sample and control any  extraneous variables that might influence your results. If if random assignment of participants to control and treatment groups is impossible, unethical, or highly difficult, consider an observational study instead.

Table of contents

Step 1: define your variables, step 2: write your hypothesis, step 3: design your experimental treatments, step 4: assign your subjects to treatment groups, step 5: measure your dependent variable, frequently asked questions about experimental design.

You should begin with a specific research question . We will work with two research question examples, one from health sciences and one from ecology:

To translate your research question into an experimental hypothesis, you need to define the main variables and make predictions about how they are related.

Start by simply listing the independent and dependent variables .

Research question Independent variable Dependent variable
Phone use and sleep Minutes of phone use before sleep Hours of sleep per night
Temperature and soil respiration Air temperature just above the soil surface CO2 respired from soil

Then you need to think about possible extraneous and confounding variables and consider how you might control  them in your experiment.

Extraneous variable How to control
Phone use and sleep in sleep patterns among individuals. measure the average difference between sleep with phone use and sleep without phone use rather than the average amount of sleep per treatment group.
Temperature and soil respiration also affects respiration, and moisture can decrease with increasing temperature. monitor soil moisture and add water to make sure that soil moisture is consistent across all treatment plots.

Finally, you can put these variables together into a diagram. Use arrows to show the possible relationships between variables and include signs to show the expected direction of the relationships.

Diagram of the relationship between variables in a sleep experiment

Here we predict that increasing temperature will increase soil respiration and decrease soil moisture, while decreasing soil moisture will lead to decreased soil respiration.

Prevent plagiarism, run a free check.

Now that you have a strong conceptual understanding of the system you are studying, you should be able to write a specific, testable hypothesis that addresses your research question.

Null hypothesis (H ) Alternate hypothesis (H )
Phone use and sleep Phone use before sleep does not correlate with the amount of sleep a person gets. Increasing phone use before sleep leads to a decrease in sleep.
Temperature and soil respiration Air temperature does not correlate with soil respiration. Increased air temperature leads to increased soil respiration.

The next steps will describe how to design a controlled experiment . In a controlled experiment, you must be able to:

  • Systematically and precisely manipulate the independent variable(s).
  • Precisely measure the dependent variable(s).
  • Control any potential confounding variables.

If your study system doesn’t match these criteria, there are other types of research you can use to answer your research question.

How you manipulate the independent variable can affect the experiment’s external validity – that is, the extent to which the results can be generalised and applied to the broader world.

First, you may need to decide how widely to vary your independent variable.

  • just slightly above the natural range for your study region.
  • over a wider range of temperatures to mimic future warming.
  • over an extreme range that is beyond any possible natural variation.

Second, you may need to choose how finely to vary your independent variable. Sometimes this choice is made for you by your experimental system, but often you will need to decide, and this will affect how much you can infer from your results.

  • a categorical variable : either as binary (yes/no) or as levels of a factor (no phone use, low phone use, high phone use).
  • a continuous variable (minutes of phone use measured every night).

How you apply your experimental treatments to your test subjects is crucial for obtaining valid and reliable results.

First, you need to consider the study size : how many individuals will be included in the experiment? In general, the more subjects you include, the greater your experiment’s statistical power , which determines how much confidence you can have in your results.

Then you need to randomly assign your subjects to treatment groups . Each group receives a different level of the treatment (e.g. no phone use, low phone use, high phone use).

You should also include a control group , which receives no treatment. The control group tells us what would have happened to your test subjects without any experimental intervention.

When assigning your subjects to groups, there are two main choices you need to make:

  • A completely randomised design vs a randomised block design .
  • A between-subjects design vs a within-subjects design .

Randomisation

An experiment can be completely randomised or randomised within blocks (aka strata):

  • In a completely randomised design , every subject is assigned to a treatment group at random.
  • In a randomised block design (aka stratified random design), subjects are first grouped according to a characteristic they share, and then randomly assigned to treatments within those groups.
Completely randomised design Randomised block design
Phone use and sleep Subjects are all randomly assigned a level of phone use using a random number generator. Subjects are first grouped by age, and then phone use treatments are randomly assigned within these groups.
Temperature and soil respiration Warming treatments are assigned to soil plots at random by using a number generator to generate map coordinates within the study area. Soils are first grouped by average rainfall, and then treatment plots are randomly assigned within these groups.

Sometimes randomisation isn’t practical or ethical , so researchers create partially-random or even non-random designs. An experimental design where treatments aren’t randomly assigned is called a quasi-experimental design .

Between-subjects vs within-subjects

In a between-subjects design (also known as an independent measures design or classic ANOVA design), individuals receive only one of the possible levels of an experimental treatment.

In medical or social research, you might also use matched pairs within your between-subjects design to make sure that each treatment group contains the same variety of test subjects in the same proportions.

In a within-subjects design (also known as a repeated measures design), every individual receives each of the experimental treatments consecutively, and their responses to each treatment are measured.

Within-subjects or repeated measures can also refer to an experimental design where an effect emerges over time, and individual responses are measured over time in order to measure this effect as it emerges.

Counterbalancing (randomising or reversing the order of treatments among subjects) is often used in within-subjects designs to ensure that the order of treatment application doesn’t influence the results of the experiment.

Between-subjects (independent measures) design Within-subjects (repeated measures) design
Phone use and sleep Subjects are randomly assigned a level of phone use (none, low, or high) and follow that level of phone use throughout the experiment. Subjects are assigned consecutively to zero, low, and high levels of phone use throughout the experiment, and the order in which they follow these treatments is randomised.
Temperature and soil respiration Warming treatments are assigned to soil plots at random and the soils are kept at this temperature throughout the experiment. Every plot receives each warming treatment (1, 3, 5, 8, and 10C above ambient temperatures) consecutively over the course of the experiment, and the order in which they receive these treatments is randomised.

Finally, you need to decide how you’ll collect data on your dependent variable outcomes. You should aim for reliable and valid measurements that minimise bias or error.

Some variables, like temperature, can be objectively measured with scientific instruments. Others may need to be operationalised to turn them into measurable observations.

  • Ask participants to record what time they go to sleep and get up each day.
  • Ask participants to wear a sleep tracker.

How precisely you measure your dependent variable also affects the kinds of statistical analysis you can use on your data.

Experiments are always context-dependent, and a good experimental design will take into account all of the unique considerations of your study system to produce information that is both valid and relevant to your research question.

Experimental designs are a set of procedures that you plan in order to examine the relationship between variables that interest you.

To design a successful experiment, first identify:

  • A testable hypothesis
  • One or more independent variables that you will manipulate
  • One or more dependent variables that you will measure

When designing the experiment, first decide:

  • How your variable(s) will be manipulated
  • How you will control for any potential confounding or lurking variables
  • How many subjects you will include
  • How you will assign treatments to your subjects

The key difference between observational studies and experiments is that, done correctly, an observational study will never influence the responses or behaviours of participants. Experimental designs will have a treatment condition applied to at least a portion of participants.

A confounding variable , also called a confounder or confounding factor, is a third variable in a study examining a potential cause-and-effect relationship.

A confounding variable is related to both the supposed cause and the supposed effect of the study. It can be difficult to separate the true effect of the independent variable from the effect of the confounding variable.

In your research design , it’s important to identify potential confounding variables and plan how you will reduce their impact.

In a between-subjects design , every participant experiences only one condition, and researchers assess group differences between participants in various conditions.

In a within-subjects design , each participant experiences all conditions, and researchers test the same participants repeatedly for differences between conditions.

The word ‘between’ means that you’re comparing different conditions between groups, while the word ‘within’ means you’re comparing different conditions within the same group.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the ‘Cite this Scribbr article’ button to automatically add the citation to our free Reference Generator.

Bevans, R. (2022, December 05). A Quick Guide to Experimental Design | 5 Steps & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved 29 August 2024, from https://www.scribbr.co.uk/research-methods/guide-to-experimental-design/

Is this article helpful?

Rebecca Bevans

Rebecca Bevans

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Published: 31 July 2018

POINTS OF SIGNIFICANCE

Optimal experimental design

  • Byran Smucker 1 ,
  • Martin Krzywinski 2 &
  • Naomi Altman 3  

Nature Methods volume  15 ,  pages 559–560 ( 2018 ) Cite this article

26k Accesses

58 Citations

21 Altmetric

Metrics details

Customize the experiment for the setting instead of adjusting the setting to fit a classical design.

You have full access to this article via your institution.

To maximize the chance for success in an experiment, good experimental design is needed. However, the presence of unique constraints may prevent mapping the experimental scenario onto a classical design. In these cases, we can use optimal design: a powerful, general-purpose tool that offers an attractive alternative to classical design and provides a framework within which to obtain high-quality, statistically grounded designs under nonstandard conditions. It can flexibly accommodate constraints, is connected to statistical quantities of interest and often mimics intuitive classical designs.

For example, suppose we wish to test the effects of a drug’s concentration in the range 0–100 ng/ml on the growth of cells. The cells will be grown with the drug in test tubes, arranged on a rack with four shelves. Our goal may be to determine whether the drug has an effect and precisely estimate the effect size or to identify the concentration at which the response is optimal. We will address both by finding designs that are optimal for regression parameter estimation as well as designs optimal for prediction precision.

To illustrate how constraints may influence our design, suppose that the shelves receive different amounts of light, which might lead to systematic variation between shelves. The shelf would therefore be a natural block 1 . Since we don’t expect such systematic variation within a shelf, the order of tubes on a shelf can be randomized. Furthermore, each shelf can only hold nine test tubes. The experimental design question, then, is: What should be the drug concentration in each of the 36 tubes?

If concentration were a categorical factor, we could compare the mean response at nine concentrations—a traditional randomized complete block design (RCBD) 1 . However, because concentration is actually continuous, discrete levels unduly limit which concentrations are studied and reduce our ability to detect an effect and estimate the concentration that produces an optimal response. Classical designs, like full factorials or RCBDs, assume an ideal and simple experimental setup, which may be inappropriate for all experimental goals or untenable in the presence of constraints.

Optimal design provides a principled approach to accommodating the entire range of concentrations and making full use of each shelf’s capacity. It can incorporate a variety of constraints such as sample size restrictions (e.g., the lab has a limited supply of test tubes), awkward blocking structures (e.g., shelves have different capacities) or disallowed treatment combinations (e.g., certain combinations of factor levels may be infeasible or otherwise undesirable).

To assist in describing optimal design, let’s review some terminology. The drug is a ‘factor’, and particular concentrations are ‘levels’. A particular combination of factor levels is a ‘treatment’ (with just a single factor, a treatment is simply a factor level) applied to an ‘experimental unit’, which is a test tube. The shelves are ‘blocks’, which are collections of experimental units that are similar in traits (e.g., light level) that might affect the experimental outcome 1 . The possible set of treatments that could be chosen is the ‘design space’. A ‘run’ is the execution of a single experimental unit, and the ‘sample size’ is the number of runs in the experiment.

Optimal design optimizes a numerical criterion, which typically relates to the variance or other statistically relevant properties of the design, and uses as input the number of runs, the factors and their possible levels, block structure (if any), and a hypothesized form of the relationship between the response and the factors. Two of the most common criteria are the D-criterion and the I-criterion. They are fundamentally different: the D-criterion relates to the variance of factor effects, and the I-criterion addresses the precision of predictions.

To understand the D-criterion (determinant), suppose we have a quadratic regression model 2 with parameters β 1 and β 2 that relate the factor to the response (for simplicity, ignore β 0 , the intercept). Our estimates of these parameters, \(\hat \beta _1\) and \(\hat \beta _2\) , will have error and, assuming the model error variance is known, the D-optimal design minimizes the area of the ellipse that defines the joint confidence interval for the parameters (Fig. 1 ). This area will include the true values of both β 1 and β 2 in 95% (or some other desired proportion) of repeated executions of the design, and its size and shape are a function of the data’s overall variance and the design.

figure 1

The ellipse can be projected onto each axis to obtain the familiar one-dimensional confidence intervals for each parameter (shown as blue points with error bars). The D-criterion reduces the variance of the parameter estimates and/or the correlation between the estimates by minimizing the area of the ellipse.

On the other hand, the I-criterion (integrated variance) is used when the experimental goal is to make precise predictions of the response, rather than to obtain precise estimates of the model parameters. An I-optimal design chooses the set of runs to minimize the average variance in prediction across the joint range of the factors. The prediction variance is a function of several elements: the data’s overall error variance, the factor levels at which we are predicting, and also the design itself. This criterion is more complicated mathematically because it involves integration.

For both criteria, numerical heuristics are used in the optimization but they do not guarantee a global optimum. For most scenarios, however, near-optimal designs are adequate and not hard to obtain.

Returning to our example, suppose we wish to obtain a precise estimate of our drug’s effect on the mean response. If we expect that the effect is linear (our model has one parameter of interest, β 1 , which is the slope), the D-optimal design places either four or five experimental units in each block at the low level (0 ng/ml) and the remaining units at the high level (100 ng/ml). Thus, to obtain a precise estimate of β 1 , we want to place the concentration values as far apart as possible in order to stabilize the estimate. Assigning four or five units of each concentration to each shelf helps to reduce the confounding of drug and shelf effects.

One downside to this simple low–high design is its inability to detect departures from linearity. If we expect that, after accounting for block differences, the relationship between the response and the factor may be curvilinear (with both a linear and quadratic term: y = β 0 + β 1 x + β 2 x 2 + ε , where ε is the error and β 0 is the intercept, which we'll ignore here; we also omit the block terms for the sake of simplicity), the D-optimal design is 3–3–3 (at 0, 50 and 100 ng/ml, respectively) within each block.

In many settings, the goal is to learn about whether and how factors affect the response (i.e., whether β 1 and/or β 2 are non-zero and, if so, how far from zero they are), in which case the D-criterion is a good choice. In other cases, the goal is to find the level of the factors that optimizes the response, in which case a design that produces more precise predictions is better. The I-criterion, which minimizes the average prediction variance across the design region, is a natural choice.

In our example, the I-optimal design for the linear model is equivalent to that generated by the D-criterion: within each block, it allocates either four or five units to the low level and the rest to the high level. However, the I-optimal design for the model that includes both linear and quadratic effects is 2–5–2 within each block; that is, it places two experimental units at the low and high levels of the factor and places five in the center.

The quality of these designs in terms of their prediction variance can be compared using fraction of design space (FDS) plots 3 . We show this plot for the D- and I-optimal designs for the quadratic case (Fig. 2a ). A point on an FDS plot gives the proportion of the design space (the fraction of the 0–100 ng/ml interval, across the blocks) that has a prediction variance less than or equal to the value on the y axis. For instance, the I-optimal design yields a lower median prediction variance than the D-optimal design: at most 0.13 for 50% of the design space as compared to 0.15. Because of the extra runs at 50 ng/ml, the I-optimal design has a lower prediction variance in the middle of the region than the D-optimal design, but variance is higher near the edges (Fig. 2b ).

figure 2

a , Prediction variance as a function of the fraction of design space (FDS). b , The variance profile across the range of concentrations for both designs.

Our one-factor blocking example demonstrates the basics of optimal design. A more realistic experiment might involve the same blocking structure but three factors—each with a specified range—and a goal to determine how the response is impacted by the factors and their interactions. We want to study the factors in combination; otherwise, any interactions between them will go undetected and the statistical efficiency to estimate factor effects is reduced.

Without the blocking constraint, a typical strategy would be to specify and use a high and low level for each factor and to perform an experiment using several replicates of the 2 3 = 8 treatment combinations. This is a classical two-level factorial design 4 that under reasonable assumptions provides ample power to detect factor effects and two-factor interactions. Unfortunately, this design doesn’t map to our scenario and can’t use the full nine-unit capacity of each shelf—unlike an optimal design, which can (Fig. 3 ).

figure 3

The D-optimal design that assigns three factors (a–c) at two levels each—low (unfilled circles) and high (filled circles)—to nine tubes on each of four shelves. The shelves are blocks and the design accounts for the main effects of the three factors and the three two-factor interactions. Each treatment is replicated at least four times, with treatments in tubes 3–7 on each shelf replicated five times.

In unconstrained settings where a classical design would be appropriate, optimal designs often turn out to be the same as their traditional counterparts. For instance, any RCBD 1 is both D- and I-optimal. Or, for a design with a sample size of 24, three factors, no blocks, and an assumed model that includes the three factor effects and all of the two-factor interactions, both the D- and I-criteria yield as optimal the two-level full-factorial design with three replicates.

So far, we have described optimal designs conceptually but have not discussed the details of how to construct them or how to analyze them 5 . Specialized software to construct optimal designs is widely available and accessible. To analyze the designs we’ve discussed—with continuous factors—it is necessary to use regression 2 (rather than ANOVA) to meaningfully relate the response to the factors. This approach allows the researcher to identify large main effects or quadratic terms and even two-factor interactions.

Optimal designs are not a panacea. There is no guarantee that (i) the experiment can achieve good power, (ii) the model form is valid and (iii) the criterion reflects the objectives of the experiment. Optimal design requires careful thought about the experiment. However, in an experiment with constraints, these assumptions can usually be specified reasonably.

Krzywinski, M. & Altman, N. Nat. Methods 11 , 699–700 (2014).

Article   PubMed   CAS   Google Scholar  

Krzywinski, M. & Altman, N. Nat. Methods 12 , 1103–1104 (2015).

Zahran, A., Anderson-Cook, C. M. & Myers, R. H. J. Qual. Tech. 35 , 377–386 (2003).

Article   Google Scholar  

Krzywinski, M. & Altman, N. Nat. Methods 11 , 1187–1188 (2014).

Goos, P. & Jones, B. Optimal Design of Experiments: A Case Study Approach (John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, UK, 2011).

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Associate Professor of Statistics at Miami University, Oxford, OH, USA

Byran Smucker

Staff scientist at Canada’s Michael Smith Genome Sciences Centre, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

Martin Krzywinski

Professor of Statistics at The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA

Naomi Altman

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martin Krzywinski .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Smucker, B., Krzywinski, M. & Altman, N. Optimal experimental design. Nat Methods 15 , 559–560 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0083-2

Download citation

Published : 31 July 2018

Issue Date : August 2018

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0083-2

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Development, optimization, and characterization of vitamin c-fortified oleogel-based chewable gels and a novel nondestructive analysis method for the vitamin c assay.

  • Reyhaneh Sabourian
  • Farid Abedin Dorkoosh
  • Mannan Hajimahmoodi

Food Production, Processing and Nutrition (2024)

Optimal experimental design for precise parameter estimation in competitive cross-reaction equilibria

  • Somaye Vali Zade
  • Hamid Abdollahi

Journal of the Iranian Chemical Society (2024)

Augmented region of interest for untargeted metabolomics mass spectrometry (AriumMS) of multi-platform-based CE-MS and LC-MS data

  • Lukas Naumann
  • Adrian Haun
  • Christian Neusüß

Analytical and Bioanalytical Chemistry (2023)

A microfluidic optimal experimental design platform for forward design of cell-free genetic networks

  • Bob van Sluijs
  • Roel J. M. Maas
  • Wilhelm T. S. Huck

Nature Communications (2022)

Advances, challenges and opportunities in creating data for trustworthy AI

  • Weixin Liang
  • Girmaw Abebe Tadesse

Nature Machine Intelligence (2022)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

experimental design application

  • Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

busayo.longe

Experimental research is the most familiar type of research design for individuals in the physical sciences and a host of other fields. This is mainly because experimental research is a classical scientific experiment, similar to those performed in high school science classes.

Imagine taking 2 samples of the same plant and exposing one of them to sunlight, while the other is kept away from sunlight. Let the plant exposed to sunlight be called sample A, while the latter is called sample B.

If after the duration of the research, we find out that sample A grows and sample B dies, even though they are both regularly wetted and given the same treatment. Therefore, we can conclude that sunlight will aid growth in all similar plants.

What is Experimental Research?

Experimental research is a scientific approach to research, where one or more independent variables are manipulated and applied to one or more dependent variables to measure their effect on the latter. The effect of the independent variables on the dependent variables is usually observed and recorded over some time, to aid researchers in drawing a reasonable conclusion regarding the relationship between these 2 variable types.

The experimental research method is widely used in physical and social sciences, psychology, and education. It is based on the comparison between two or more groups with a straightforward logic, which may, however, be difficult to execute.

Mostly related to a laboratory test procedure, experimental research designs involve collecting quantitative data and performing statistical analysis on them during research. Therefore, making it an example of quantitative research method .

What are The Types of Experimental Research Design?

The types of experimental research design are determined by the way the researcher assigns subjects to different conditions and groups. They are of 3 types, namely; pre-experimental, quasi-experimental, and true experimental research.

Pre-experimental Research Design

In pre-experimental research design, either a group or various dependent groups are observed for the effect of the application of an independent variable which is presumed to cause change. It is the simplest form of experimental research design and is treated with no control group.

Although very practical, experimental research is lacking in several areas of the true-experimental criteria. The pre-experimental research design is further divided into three types

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design

In this type of experimental study, only one dependent group or variable is considered. The study is carried out after some treatment which was presumed to cause change, making it a posttest study.

  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design: 

This research design combines both posttest and pretest study by carrying out a test on a single group before the treatment is administered and after the treatment is administered. With the former being administered at the beginning of treatment and later at the end.

  • Static-group Comparison: 

In a static-group comparison study, 2 or more groups are placed under observation, where only one of the groups is subjected to some treatment while the other groups are held static. All the groups are post-tested, and the observed differences between the groups are assumed to be a result of the treatment.

Quasi-experimental Research Design

  The word “quasi” means partial, half, or pseudo. Therefore, the quasi-experimental research bearing a resemblance to the true experimental research, but not the same.  In quasi-experiments, the participants are not randomly assigned, and as such, they are used in settings where randomization is difficult or impossible.

 This is very common in educational research, where administrators are unwilling to allow the random selection of students for experimental samples.

Some examples of quasi-experimental research design include; the time series, no equivalent control group design, and the counterbalanced design.

True Experimental Research Design

The true experimental research design relies on statistical analysis to approve or disprove a hypothesis. It is the most accurate type of experimental design and may be carried out with or without a pretest on at least 2 randomly assigned dependent subjects.

The true experimental research design must contain a control group, a variable that can be manipulated by the researcher, and the distribution must be random. The classification of true experimental design include:

  • The posttest-only Control Group Design: In this design, subjects are randomly selected and assigned to the 2 groups (control and experimental), and only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested, and a conclusion is drawn from the difference between these groups.
  • The pretest-posttest Control Group Design: For this control group design, subjects are randomly assigned to the 2 groups, both are presented, but only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested to measure the degree of change in each group.
  • Solomon four-group Design: This is the combination of the pretest-only and the pretest-posttest control groups. In this case, the randomly selected subjects are placed into 4 groups.

The first two of these groups are tested using the posttest-only method, while the other two are tested using the pretest-posttest method.

Examples of Experimental Research

Experimental research examples are different, depending on the type of experimental research design that is being considered. The most basic example of experimental research is laboratory experiments, which may differ in nature depending on the subject of research.

Administering Exams After The End of Semester

During the semester, students in a class are lectured on particular courses and an exam is administered at the end of the semester. In this case, the students are the subjects or dependent variables while the lectures are the independent variables treated on the subjects.

Only one group of carefully selected subjects are considered in this research, making it a pre-experimental research design example. We will also notice that tests are only carried out at the end of the semester, and not at the beginning.

Further making it easy for us to conclude that it is a one-shot case study research. 

Employee Skill Evaluation

Before employing a job seeker, organizations conduct tests that are used to screen out less qualified candidates from the pool of qualified applicants. This way, organizations can determine an employee’s skill set at the point of employment.

In the course of employment, organizations also carry out employee training to improve employee productivity and generally grow the organization. Further evaluation is carried out at the end of each training to test the impact of the training on employee skills, and test for improvement.

Here, the subject is the employee, while the treatment is the training conducted. This is a pretest-posttest control group experimental research example.

Evaluation of Teaching Method

Let us consider an academic institution that wants to evaluate the teaching method of 2 teachers to determine which is best. Imagine a case whereby the students assigned to each teacher is carefully selected probably due to personal request by parents or due to stubbornness and smartness.

This is a no equivalent group design example because the samples are not equal. By evaluating the effectiveness of each teacher’s teaching method this way, we may conclude after a post-test has been carried out.

However, this may be influenced by factors like the natural sweetness of a student. For example, a very smart student will grab more easily than his or her peers irrespective of the method of teaching.

What are the Characteristics of Experimental Research?  

Experimental research contains dependent, independent and extraneous variables. The dependent variables are the variables being treated or manipulated and are sometimes called the subject of the research.

The independent variables are the experimental treatment being exerted on the dependent variables. Extraneous variables, on the other hand, are other factors affecting the experiment that may also contribute to the change.

The setting is where the experiment is carried out. Many experiments are carried out in the laboratory, where control can be exerted on the extraneous variables, thereby eliminating them.

Other experiments are carried out in a less controllable setting. The choice of setting used in research depends on the nature of the experiment being carried out.

  • Multivariable

Experimental research may include multiple independent variables, e.g. time, skills, test scores, etc.

Why Use Experimental Research Design?  

Experimental research design can be majorly used in physical sciences, social sciences, education, and psychology. It is used to make predictions and draw conclusions on a subject matter. 

Some uses of experimental research design are highlighted below.

  • Medicine: Experimental research is used to provide the proper treatment for diseases. In most cases, rather than directly using patients as the research subject, researchers take a sample of the bacteria from the patient’s body and are treated with the developed antibacterial

The changes observed during this period are recorded and evaluated to determine its effectiveness. This process can be carried out using different experimental research methods.

  • Education: Asides from science subjects like Chemistry and Physics which involves teaching students how to perform experimental research, it can also be used in improving the standard of an academic institution. This includes testing students’ knowledge on different topics, coming up with better teaching methods, and the implementation of other programs that will aid student learning.
  • Human Behavior: Social scientists are the ones who mostly use experimental research to test human behaviour. For example, consider 2 people randomly chosen to be the subject of the social interaction research where one person is placed in a room without human interaction for 1 year.

The other person is placed in a room with a few other people, enjoying human interaction. There will be a difference in their behaviour at the end of the experiment.

  • UI/UX: During the product development phase, one of the major aims of the product team is to create a great user experience with the product. Therefore, before launching the final product design, potential are brought in to interact with the product.

For example, when finding it difficult to choose how to position a button or feature on the app interface, a random sample of product testers are allowed to test the 2 samples and how the button positioning influences the user interaction is recorded.

What are the Disadvantages of Experimental Research?  

  • It is highly prone to human error due to its dependency on variable control which may not be properly implemented. These errors could eliminate the validity of the experiment and the research being conducted.
  • Exerting control of extraneous variables may create unrealistic situations. Eliminating real-life variables will result in inaccurate conclusions. This may also result in researchers controlling the variables to suit his or her personal preferences.
  • It is a time-consuming process. So much time is spent on testing dependent variables and waiting for the effect of the manipulation of dependent variables to manifest.
  • It is expensive.
  • It is very risky and may have ethical complications that cannot be ignored. This is common in medical research, where failed trials may lead to a patient’s death or a deteriorating health condition.
  • Experimental research results are not descriptive.
  • Response bias can also be supplied by the subject of the conversation.
  • Human responses in experimental research can be difficult to measure.

What are the Data Collection Methods in Experimental Research?  

Data collection methods in experimental research are the different ways in which data can be collected for experimental research. They are used in different cases, depending on the type of research being carried out.

1. Observational Study

This type of study is carried out over a long period. It measures and observes the variables of interest without changing existing conditions.

When researching the effect of social interaction on human behavior, the subjects who are placed in 2 different environments are observed throughout the research. No matter the kind of absurd behavior that is exhibited by the subject during this period, its condition will not be changed.

This may be a very risky thing to do in medical cases because it may lead to death or worse medical conditions.

2. Simulations

This procedure uses mathematical, physical, or computer models to replicate a real-life process or situation. It is frequently used when the actual situation is too expensive, dangerous, or impractical to replicate in real life.

This method is commonly used in engineering and operational research for learning purposes and sometimes as a tool to estimate possible outcomes of real research. Some common situation software are Simulink, MATLAB, and Simul8.

Not all kinds of experimental research can be carried out using simulation as a data collection tool . It is very impractical for a lot of laboratory-based research that involves chemical processes.

A survey is a tool used to gather relevant data about the characteristics of a population and is one of the most common data collection tools. A survey consists of a group of questions prepared by the researcher, to be answered by the research subject.

Surveys can be shared with the respondents both physically and electronically. When collecting data through surveys, the kind of data collected depends on the respondent, and researchers have limited control over it.

Formplus is the best tool for collecting experimental data using survey s. It has relevant features that will aid the data collection process and can also be used in other aspects of experimental research.

Differences between Experimental and Non-Experimental Research 

1. In experimental research, the researcher can control and manipulate the environment of the research, including the predictor variable which can be changed. On the other hand, non-experimental research cannot be controlled or manipulated by the researcher at will.

This is because it takes place in a real-life setting, where extraneous variables cannot be eliminated. Therefore, it is more difficult to conclude non-experimental studies, even though they are much more flexible and allow for a greater range of study fields.

2. The relationship between cause and effect cannot be established in non-experimental research, while it can be established in experimental research. This may be because many extraneous variables also influence the changes in the research subject, making it difficult to point at a particular variable as the cause of a particular change

3. Independent variables are not introduced, withdrawn, or manipulated in non-experimental designs, but the same may not be said about experimental research.

Experimental Research vs. Alternatives and When to Use Them

1. experimental research vs causal comparative.

Experimental research enables you to control variables and identify how the independent variable affects the dependent variable. Causal-comparative find out the cause-and-effect relationship between the variables by comparing already existing groups that are affected differently by the independent variable.

For example, in an experiment to see how K-12 education affects children and teenager development. An experimental research would split the children into groups, some would get formal K-12 education, while others won’t. This is not ethically right because every child has the right to education. So, what we do instead would be to compare already existing groups of children who are getting formal education with those who due to some circumstances can not.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Causal-Comparative Research

  • Causal-Comparative:   Strengths:  More realistic than experiments, can be conducted in real-world settings.  Weaknesses:  Establishing causality can be weaker due to the lack of manipulation.

2. Experimental Research vs Correlational Research

When experimenting, you are trying to establish a cause-and-effect relationship between different variables. For example, you are trying to establish the effect of heat on water, the temperature keeps changing (independent variable) and you see how it affects the water (dependent variable).

For correlational research, you are not necessarily interested in the why or the cause-and-effect relationship between the variables, you are focusing on the relationship. Using the same water and temperature example, you are only interested in the fact that they change, you are not investigating which of the variables or other variables causes them to change.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Correlational Research

3. experimental research vs descriptive research.

With experimental research, you alter the independent variable to see how it affects the dependent variable, but with descriptive research you are simply studying the characteristics of the variable you are studying.

So, in an experiment to see how blown glass reacts to temperature, experimental research would keep altering the temperature to varying levels of high and low to see how it affects the dependent variable (glass). But descriptive research would investigate the glass properties.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Descriptive Research

4. experimental research vs action research.

Experimental research tests for causal relationships by focusing on one independent variable vs the dependent variable and keeps other variables constant. So, you are testing hypotheses and using the information from the research to contribute to knowledge.

However, with action research, you are using a real-world setting which means you are not controlling variables. You are also performing the research to solve actual problems and improve already established practices.

For example, if you are testing for how long commutes affect workers’ productivity. With experimental research, you would vary the length of commute to see how the time affects work. But with action research, you would account for other factors such as weather, commute route, nutrition, etc. Also, experimental research helps know the relationship between commute time and productivity, while action research helps you look for ways to improve productivity

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Action Research

Conclusion  .

Experimental research designs are often considered to be the standard in research designs. This is partly due to the common misconception that research is equivalent to scientific experiments—a component of experimental research design.

In this research design, one or more subjects or dependent variables are randomly assigned to different treatments (i.e. independent variables manipulated by the researcher) and the results are observed to conclude. One of the uniqueness of experimental research is in its ability to control the effect of extraneous variables.

Experimental research is suitable for research whose goal is to examine cause-effect relationships, e.g. explanatory research. It can be conducted in the laboratory or field settings, depending on the aim of the research that is being carried out. 

Logo

Connect to Formplus, Get Started Now - It's Free!

  • examples of experimental research
  • experimental research methods
  • types of experimental research
  • busayo.longe

Formplus

You may also like:

What is Experimenter Bias? Definition, Types & Mitigation

In this article, we will look into the concept of experimental bias and how it can be identified in your research

experimental design application

Response vs Explanatory Variables: Definition & Examples

In this article, we’ll be comparing the two types of variables, what they both mean and see some of their real-life applications in research

Simpson’s Paradox & How to Avoid it in Experimental Research

In this article, we are going to look at Simpson’s Paradox from its historical point and later, we’ll consider its effect in...

Experimental Vs Non-Experimental Research: 15 Key Differences

Differences between experimental and non experimental research on definitions, types, examples, data collection tools, uses, advantages etc.

Formplus - For Seamless Data Collection

Collect data the right way with a versatile data collection tool. try formplus and transform your work productivity today..

Experimental Design: Types, Examples & Methods

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Experimental design refers to how participants are allocated to different groups in an experiment. Types of design include repeated measures, independent groups, and matched pairs designs.

Probably the most common way to design an experiment in psychology is to divide the participants into two groups, the experimental group and the control group, and then introduce a change to the experimental group, not the control group.

The researcher must decide how he/she will allocate their sample to the different experimental groups.  For example, if there are 10 participants, will all 10 participants participate in both groups (e.g., repeated measures), or will the participants be split in half and take part in only one group each?

Three types of experimental designs are commonly used:

1. Independent Measures

Independent measures design, also known as between-groups , is an experimental design where different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable.  This means that each condition of the experiment includes a different group of participants.

This should be done by random allocation, ensuring that each participant has an equal chance of being assigned to one group.

Independent measures involve using two separate groups of participants, one in each condition. For example:

Independent Measures Design 2

  • Con : More people are needed than with the repeated measures design (i.e., more time-consuming).
  • Pro : Avoids order effects (such as practice or fatigue) as people participate in one condition only.  If a person is involved in several conditions, they may become bored, tired, and fed up by the time they come to the second condition or become wise to the requirements of the experiment!
  • Con : Differences between participants in the groups may affect results, for example, variations in age, gender, or social background.  These differences are known as participant variables (i.e., a type of extraneous variable ).
  • Control : After the participants have been recruited, they should be randomly assigned to their groups. This should ensure the groups are similar, on average (reducing participant variables).

2. Repeated Measures Design

Repeated Measures design is an experimental design where the same participants participate in each independent variable condition.  This means that each experiment condition includes the same group of participants.

Repeated Measures design is also known as within-groups or within-subjects design .

  • Pro : As the same participants are used in each condition, participant variables (i.e., individual differences) are reduced.
  • Con : There may be order effects. Order effects refer to the order of the conditions affecting the participants’ behavior.  Performance in the second condition may be better because the participants know what to do (i.e., practice effect).  Or their performance might be worse in the second condition because they are tired (i.e., fatigue effect). This limitation can be controlled using counterbalancing.
  • Pro : Fewer people are needed as they participate in all conditions (i.e., saves time).
  • Control : To combat order effects, the researcher counter-balances the order of the conditions for the participants.  Alternating the order in which participants perform in different conditions of an experiment.

Counterbalancing

Suppose we used a repeated measures design in which all of the participants first learned words in “loud noise” and then learned them in “no noise.”

We expect the participants to learn better in “no noise” because of order effects, such as practice. However, a researcher can control for order effects using counterbalancing.

The sample would be split into two groups: experimental (A) and control (B).  For example, group 1 does ‘A’ then ‘B,’ and group 2 does ‘B’ then ‘A.’ This is to eliminate order effects.

Although order effects occur for each participant, they balance each other out in the results because they occur equally in both groups.

counter balancing

3. Matched Pairs Design

A matched pairs design is an experimental design where pairs of participants are matched in terms of key variables, such as age or socioeconomic status. One member of each pair is then placed into the experimental group and the other member into the control group .

One member of each matched pair must be randomly assigned to the experimental group and the other to the control group.

matched pairs design

  • Con : If one participant drops out, you lose 2 PPs’ data.
  • Pro : Reduces participant variables because the researcher has tried to pair up the participants so that each condition has people with similar abilities and characteristics.
  • Con : Very time-consuming trying to find closely matched pairs.
  • Pro : It avoids order effects, so counterbalancing is not necessary.
  • Con : Impossible to match people exactly unless they are identical twins!
  • Control : Members of each pair should be randomly assigned to conditions. However, this does not solve all these problems.

Experimental design refers to how participants are allocated to an experiment’s different conditions (or IV levels). There are three types:

1. Independent measures / between-groups : Different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable.

2. Repeated measures /within groups : The same participants take part in each condition of the independent variable.

3. Matched pairs : Each condition uses different participants, but they are matched in terms of important characteristics, e.g., gender, age, intelligence, etc.

Learning Check

Read about each of the experiments below. For each experiment, identify (1) which experimental design was used; and (2) why the researcher might have used that design.

1 . To compare the effectiveness of two different types of therapy for depression, depressed patients were assigned to receive either cognitive therapy or behavior therapy for a 12-week period.

The researchers attempted to ensure that the patients in the two groups had similar severity of depressed symptoms by administering a standardized test of depression to each participant, then pairing them according to the severity of their symptoms.

2 . To assess the difference in reading comprehension between 7 and 9-year-olds, a researcher recruited each group from a local primary school. They were given the same passage of text to read and then asked a series of questions to assess their understanding.

3 . To assess the effectiveness of two different ways of teaching reading, a group of 5-year-olds was recruited from a primary school. Their level of reading ability was assessed, and then they were taught using scheme one for 20 weeks.

At the end of this period, their reading was reassessed, and a reading improvement score was calculated. They were then taught using scheme two for a further 20 weeks, and another reading improvement score for this period was calculated. The reading improvement scores for each child were then compared.

4 . To assess the effect of the organization on recall, a researcher randomly assigned student volunteers to two conditions.

Condition one attempted to recall a list of words that were organized into meaningful categories; condition two attempted to recall the same words, randomly grouped on the page.

Experiment Terminology

Ecological validity.

The degree to which an investigation represents real-life experiences.

Experimenter effects

These are the ways that the experimenter can accidentally influence the participant through their appearance or behavior.

Demand characteristics

The clues in an experiment lead the participants to think they know what the researcher is looking for (e.g., the experimenter’s body language).

Independent variable (IV)

The variable the experimenter manipulates (i.e., changes) is assumed to have a direct effect on the dependent variable.

Dependent variable (DV)

Variable the experimenter measures. This is the outcome (i.e., the result) of a study.

Extraneous variables (EV)

All variables which are not independent variables but could affect the results (DV) of the experiment. Extraneous variables should be controlled where possible.

Confounding variables

Variable(s) that have affected the results (DV), apart from the IV. A confounding variable could be an extraneous variable that has not been controlled.

Random Allocation

Randomly allocating participants to independent variable conditions means that all participants should have an equal chance of taking part in each condition.

The principle of random allocation is to avoid bias in how the experiment is carried out and limit the effects of participant variables.

Order effects

Changes in participants’ performance due to their repeating the same or similar test more than once. Examples of order effects include:

(i) practice effect: an improvement in performance on a task due to repetition, for example, because of familiarity with the task;

(ii) fatigue effect: a decrease in performance of a task due to repetition, for example, because of boredom or tiredness.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

IEEE Account

  • Change Username/Password
  • Update Address

Purchase Details

  • Payment Options
  • Order History
  • View Purchased Documents

Profile Information

  • Communications Preferences
  • Profession and Education
  • Technical Interests
  • US & Canada: +1 800 678 4333
  • Worldwide: +1 732 981 0060
  • Contact & Support
  • About IEEE Xplore
  • Accessibility
  • Terms of Use
  • Nondiscrimination Policy
  • Privacy & Opting Out of Cookies

A not-for-profit organization, IEEE is the world's largest technical professional organization dedicated to advancing technology for the benefit of humanity. © Copyright 2024 IEEE - All rights reserved. Use of this web site signifies your agreement to the terms and conditions.

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

applsci-logo

Article Menu

experimental design application

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Experimental analysis and design of 3d-printed polymer elliptical tubes in compression.

experimental design application

Featured Application

1. introduction, 2. material testing, 2.1. tensile testing, 2.2. compression testing, 2.3. poisson’s ratio in compression, 3. full-scale testing, 3.1. elliptical tube geometry, 3.2. fabrication and precision, 3.3. test methodology, 3.4. results, 3.4.1. load–end shortening relationships, 3.4.2. failure and deformation modes, 3.4.3. longitudinal strains, 4. design method, 4.1. local buckling of elliptical sections, 4.2. design method for slender elliptical tubes in compression, 5. conclusions, author contributions, data availability statement, acknowledgments, conflicts of interest.

  • Rodríguez-Panes, A.; Claver, J.; Camacho, A.M. The Influence of Manufacturing Parameters on the Mechanical Behaviour of PLA and ABS Pieces Manufactured by FDM: A Comparative Analysis. Materials 2018 , 11 , 1333. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Farah, S.; Anderson, D.; Langer, R. Physical and mechanical properties of PLA, and their functions in widespread applications—A comprehensive review. Adv. Drug Deliv. Rev. 2016 , 107 , 367–392. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Torre, R.; Brischetto, S.; Dipietro, I.R. Buckling developed in 3D printed PLA cuboidal samples under compression: Analytical, numerical and experimental investigations. Addit. Manuf. 2021 , 38 , 101790. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Roy, R.; Mukhopadhyay, A. Tribological studies of 3D printed ABS and PLA plastic parts. Mater. Today Proc. 2021 , 41 , 856–862. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hsueh, M.H.; Lai, C.J.; Wang, S.H.; Zeng, Y.S.; Hsieh, C.H.; Pan, C.Y.; Huang, W.C. Effect of printing parameters on the thermal and mechanical properties of 3D-Printed PLA and PETG using Fused Deposition Modeling. Polymers 2021 , 3 , 1758. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • UM180821 TDS PLA RB V11 Technical Data Sheet PLA ; Ultimaker: Zaltbommel, The Netherlands, 2018.
  • Technical Data Sheet—ABSX ; MCPP: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018.
  • Almkvist, G.; Berndt, B. Gauss, Landen, Ramanujan, the Arithmetic-geometric Mean, Ellipses, π, and the Ladies Diary. Amer. Math. Mon. 1988 , 95 , 585–608. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhou, C.; Hu, Y.; Zhang, L.; Fang, J.; Xi, Y.; Hu, J.; Li, Y.; Liu, L.; Zhao, Y.; Yang, L.; et al. Investigation of heat transfer characteristics of deflected elliptical-tube heat exchanger in closed-circuit cooling towers. Appl. Therm. Eng. 2024 , 236 , 121860. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Khuda, M.A.; Sarunac, N. A comparative study of latent heat thermal energy storage (LTES) system using cylindrical and elliptical tubes in a staggered tube arrangement. J. Energy Storage 2024 , 87 , 111333. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Boiko, A.V.; Demyanko, K.V. On numerical stability analysis of fluid flows in compliant pipes of elliptic cross-section. J. Fluids Struct. 2022 , 108 , 103414. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shi, X.; Alam, M.M.; Zhu, H.; Ji, C.; Bai, H.; Sharifpur, M. Flow three-dimensionality of wavy elliptic cylinder: Vortex shedding bifurcation. Ocean. Eng. 2024 , 301 , 117527. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ruiz-Teran, A.M.; Gardner, L. Elastic buckling of elliptical tubes. Thin-Walled Struct. 2008 , 46 , 1304–1318. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kempner, J.; Chen, Y.N. Large deflections of an axially compressed oval cylindrical shell. In Proceedings of the 11th International Congress of Applied Mechanics, Munich, Germany, 30 August–5 September 1964. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feinstein, G.; Erickson, B.; Kempner, J. Stability of oval cylindrical shells. Exp. Mech. 1971 , 11 , 514–520. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Silvestre, N.; Gardner, L. Elastic local postbuckling of elliptical tubes. J. Const. Steel Res. 2011 , 67 , 281–292. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Insausti, A.; Gardner, L. Analytical modelling of plastic collapse in compressed elliptical hollow sections. J. Const. Steel Res. 2011 , 67 , 678–689. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Abela, J.M.; Gardner, L. Elastic buckling of elliptical tubes subjected to generalised linearly varying stress distributions. Thin-Walled Struct. 2012 , 58 , 40–50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yao, Y.; Quach, W.-M.; Young, B. Strength enhancement and stub-column behavior of cold-formed stainless steel elliptical hollow sections. Thin-Walled Struct. 2023 , 189 , 110939. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ge, C.; Gao, Q.; Wang, L.; Hong, Z. Theoretical prediction and numerical analysis for axial crushing behaviour of elliptical aluminium foam-filled tube. Thin-Walled Struct. 2020 , 149 , 106523. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Al-saadi, A.U.; Thiru Aravinthan, T.; Lokuge, W. Structural applications of fibre reinforced polymer (FRP)composite tubes: A review of columns members. Compos. Struct. 2018 , 204 , 513–524. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McCann, F.; Fang, C.; Gardner, L.; Silvestre, N. Local buckling and ultimate strength of slender elliptical hollow sections in compression. Eng. Struct. 2016 , 111 , 104–118. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McCann, F.; Gardner, L. Numerical analysis and design of slender elliptical hollow sections in bending. Thin-Walled Struct. 2019 , 139 , 196–208. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vukasovic, T.; Vivanco, J.F.; Celentano, D.; García-Herrera, C. Characterization of the mechanical response of thermoplastic parts fabricated with 3D printing. Int. J. Adv. Manuf. Technol. 2019 , 104 , 4207–4218. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dundar, M.A.; Dhaliwal, G.S.; Ayorinde, E.; Al-Zubi, M. Tensile, compression, and flexural characteristics of acrylonitrile–butadiene–styrene at low strain rates: Experimental and numerical investigation. Polym. Polym. Compos. 2021 , 29 , 331–342. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ansari, A.A.; Kamil, M. Effect of print speed and extrusion temperature on properties of 3D printed PLA used fused deposition modeling process. Mater. Today Proc. 2021 , 45 , 5462–5468. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ayatollahi, M.R.; Nabavi-Kivi, A.; Bahrami, B.; Yahya, M.Y.; Khosravani, M.R. The influence of in-plane raster angle on tensile and fracture strengths of 3D-printed PLA specimens. Eng. Fract. Mech. 2020 , 237 , 107225. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kiendl, J.; Gao, C. Controlling toughness and strength of FDM 3D-printed PLA components through the raster layup. Compos. Part B Eng. 2020 , 180 , 107562. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Guan, Y.; Virgin, L.N.; Helm, D. Structural behavior of shallow geodesic lattice domes. Int. J. Solids Struct. 2018 , 155 , 225–239. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Virgin, L.N.; Guan, Y.; Plaut, R.H. On the geometric conditions for multiple stable equilibria in clamped arches. Int. J. Non-Linear Mech. 2017 , 92 , 8–14. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nomani, J.; Wilson, D.; Paulino, M.; Mohammed, M.I. Effect of layer thickness and cross-section geometry on the tensile and compression properties of 3D printed ABS. Mater. Today Commun. 2020 , 22 , 100626. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Grant, A.; Regez, B.; Kocak, S.; Huber, J.D.; Mooers, A. Anisotropic properties of 3-D printed Poly Lactic Acid (PLA) and Acrylonitrile Butadiene Styrene (ABS) plastics. Results Mater. 2021 , 12 , 100227. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • EN ISO 527-2 ; Plastics—Determination of Tensile Properties—Part 2: Test Conditions for Moulding and Extrusion Plastics. CEN: Meyrin, Switzerland, 1996.
  • Prusa. PrusaSlicer 2.4.0 Manual. 2021. Available online: https://help.prusa3d.com/en/article/general-info_1910 (accessed on 13 December 2021).
  • EN ISO 527-1 ; Plastics—Determination of Tensile Properties—Part 1: General Principles. CEN: Meyrin, Switzerland, 1993.
  • ASTM D695 ; Standard Test Method for Compressive Properties of Rigid Plastics. ASTM: West Conshohocken, PA, USA, 2016.
  • Ashby, M.F. The mechanical properties of cellular solids. Metall. Trans. A 1983 , 14 , 1755–1769. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ramberg, W.; Osgood, W.R. Description of Stress–Strain Curves by Three Parameters. Technical Note No. 902 ; National Advisory Committee for Aeronautics: Washington, DC, USA, 1943. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferreira, R.T.L.; Cardoso Amatte, I.; Dutra, T.A.; Bürger, D. Experimental characterization and micrography of 3D printed PLA and PLA reinforced with short carbon fibers. Compos. Part B Eng. 2017 , 124 , 88–100. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zou, R.; Xia, Y.; Liu, S.; Hu, P.; Hou, W.; Hu, Q.; Shan, C. Isotropic and anisotropic elasticity and yielding of 3D printed material. Compos. Part B Eng. 2016 , 99 , 506–513. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McCann, F.; Rossi, F. Investigating local buckling in highly slender elliptical hollow sections through analysis of 3D-printed analogues. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Coupled Instabilities in Metal Structures, Łodz, Poland, 12–14 July 2021. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ree, T.; Eyring, H. Theory of Non-Newtonian Flow. I. Solid Plastic System. J. Appl. Phys. 1955 , 26 , 793–800. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verbeeten, W.M.H.; Lorenzo-Bañuelos, M.; Arribas-Subiñas, P.J. Anisotropic rate-dependent mechanical behavior of Poly (Lactic Acid) processed by Material Extrusion Additive Manufacturing. Addit. Manuf. 2020 , 31 , 100968. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gardner, L.; Chan, T.M. Cross-section classification of elliptical hollow sections. Steel Comp. Struct. 2007 , 7 , 185–200. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chan, T.M.; Gardner, L. Compressive resistance of hot-rolled elliptical hollow sections. Eng. Struct. 2008 , 30 , 522–532. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • EN 1993-1-1 ; Eurocode 3: Design of Steel Structure—Part 1–1: General Rules for Buildings. British Standards Institute. CEN: Meyrin, Switzerland, 2024.
  • EN 1993-1-4 ; Eurocode 3: Design of Steel Structures—Part 1–4: General Rules—Supplementary Rules for Stainless Steels. British Standards Institute. CEN: Meyrin, Switzerland, 2006.
  • Tsai, S.W.; Wu, E.M. A general theory of strength for anisotropic materials. In Technical Report AFML-TR-71-12, August 1972 ; Air Force Materials Laboratory: Dayton, OH, USA, 1972. [ Google Scholar ]

Click here to enlarge figure

MaterialStrength
(N/mm )
Elastic Modulus
(N/mm )
Density
(kg/m )
Normalised Strength-to-Weight Ratio
(km)
Ultimaker PLA [ ]50235012484.08
MCPP ABSX [ ]39198010303.86
S275 steel275210,00078503.57
S355 steel355210,00078504.61
C40 concrete4032,10024001.70
C24 timber2410,8005504.45
Specimenb b COV(b )COV(b )E σ ε ε
(mm)(mm)(%)(%)(N/mm )(N/mm )(%)(%)
PLA
  T01-PLA-X-19.924.200.946.03277440.71.481.48
  T02-PLA-X-29.954.200.596.23279551.51.881.88
  T03-PLA-X-39.914.181.075.53283453.12.102.13
  T04-PLA-Y-110.913.9811.111.30308953.41.931.93
  T05-PLA-Y-210.873.9810.621.17321350.61.761.76
  T06-PLA-Y-310.903.9811.031.16323153.91.901.91
  T07-PLA-Z-110.354.354.3310.84306937.61.331.33
  T08-PLA-Z-210.384.404.6212.15310646.21.871.87
  T09-PLA-Z-310.334.364.0811.03296844.91.761.76
ABS
  T10-ABS-X-110.134.191.695.96169231.12.382.90
  T11-ABS-X-29.854.161.884.85177232.12.322.49
  T12-ABS-X-39.864.171.765.23177031.92.332.66
  T13-ABS-Y-110.543.966.571.25173934.02.382.40
  T14-ABS-Y-210.533.986.490.79173733.62.342.35
  T15-ABS-Y-310.594.017.270.43173433.02.302.32
  T16-ABS-Z-110.094.081.422.88186615.40.920.92
  T17-ABS-Z-210.094.071.222.59159917.41.131.16
  T18-ABS-Z-39.934.161.214.86168418.01.171.17
Material OrientationCOV(b )COV(b )E COV(E )σ COV(σ )ε ε
(%)(%)(N/mm )(%)(N/mm )(%)(%)(%)
PLA
 X orientation0.775.1428010.9848.413.91.821.83
 Y orientation9.461.0531782.4352.63.381.871.87
 Z orientation3.779.8330482.3442.910.811.651.65
ABS
 X orientation1.544.6517452.6131.71.672.342.68
 Y orientation5.880.7717370.1433.51.502.342.36
 Z orientation1.123.1117167.9517.08.041.071.08
Specimenb b COV(b )COV(b )E σ σ ε
(mm)(mm)(%)(%)(N/mm )(N/mm )(N/mm )(%)
PLA batch 1
  C01-PLA1-X-112.8412.561.321.32251658.358.43.53
  C02-PLA1-X-212.8512.641.450.63267562.062.13.76
  C03-PLA1-X-312.8412.581.321.19278262.963.63.25
  C04-PLA1-X-412.9312.602.460.93245459.059.03.39
  C05-PLA1-Z-113.0112.982.712.99240172.073.73.57
  C06-PLA1-Z-212.7612.780.940.68269975.175.13.57
  C07-PLA1-Z-312.7212.730.330.30298776.476.93.37
PLA batch 2
  C08-PLA2-X-112.5112.721.800.24271572.572.93.61
  C09-PLA2-X-212.5012.751.900.53249073.373.73.87
  C10-PLA2-X-312.4812.742.160.45274772.573.33.68
  C11-PLA2-Z-112.5312.851.641.42313576.577.73.55
  C12-PLA2-Z-212.5212.791.740.91310876.177.43.59
  C13-PLA2-Z-312.5612.841.391.42296873.874.33.42
ABS
  C14-ABS-X-112.9212.572.131.26130138.5-4.01
  C15-ABS-X-212.8912.702.040.06117437.5-4.23
  C16-ABS-X-312.7412.480.532.18142039.6-3.83
  C17-ABS-X-412.9412.672.540.29106040.1-4.90
  C18-ABS-Z-112.6112.680.920.73141140.740.83.80
  C19-ABS-Z-212.6212.690.800.25149241.141.13.77
  C20-ABS-Z-312.6212.590.871.12156041.241.23.61
  C21-ABS-Z-412.5912.681.120.41155941.541.53.62
SpecimenCOV(b )COV(b )E COV(E )σ COV(σ )σ ε
(%)(%)(N/mm )(%)(N/mm )(%)(N/mm )(%)
PLA batch 1
 X orientation 1.450.9026075.7360.63.7060.83.48
 Z orientation1.441.54269610.8774.53.0375.23.51
PLA batch 2
 X orientation 1.700.3726515.2872.80.6373.33.72
 Z orientation0.821.1230702.9275.51.9376.53.52
ABS
 X orientation 1.681.08123912.5838.92.98-4.25
 Z orientation0.800.6115064.6941.10.8041.13.70
Materialν
PLA
 X orientation 0.322
 Z orientation0.325
ABS
 X orientation 0.341
 Z orientation0.332
Specimen2a2ba/bWall Thickness tP A
NominalAverageSt. DevCOV
(mm)(mm)(mm)(mm)(mm)(%)(mm)(mm )
EHS01-PLA1-100-50-3.0100.1450.012.03.003.170.113.48233737
EHS02-PLA1-90-60-2.090.1760.091.52.002.070.083.96232478
EHS03-PLA1-90-60-2.090.0260.111.52.002.080.062.73232481
EHS04-PLA1-90-60-2.090.0660.081.52.002.040.094.35232473
EHS05-PLA1-100-50-3.0100.0450.052.03.003.100.123.93233721
EHS06-PLA1-100-50-1.5100.0650.082.01.501.450.075.02238344
EHS07-PLA1-105-35-2.0105.1335.073.02.002.020.104.73228459
EHS08-PLA1-105-35-1.5105.0835.013.01.501.390.1510.91230344
EHS09-PLA2-90-60-1.090.1559.711.51.001.130.032.65234235
EHS10-PLA2-90-60-1.590.0260.091.51.501.600.053.21233350
EHS11-PLA2-90-60-3.090.0360.011.53.003.030.051.63228686
EHS12-PLA2-100-50-1.0100.0850.032.01.001.130.054.07238239
EHS13-PLA2-100-50-2.0100.0650.012.02.002.260.073.16235472
EHS14-PLA2-100-50-2.5100.0350.042.02.502.730.062.19233586
EHS15-PLA2-105-35-1.0105.0535.033.01.001.260.043.45230231
EHS16-PLA2-105-35-2.5105.0735.013.02.502.720.051.94225566
EHS17-PLA2-105-35-3.0105.0235.013.03.003.200.072.03224675
EHS18-ABS-100-50-1.099.7050.652.01.001.210.053.71239239
EHS19-ABS-90-60-1.090.1659.981.51.501.160.075.67235235
EHS20-ABS-90-60-1.590.0760.141.51.001.790.052.87233464
EHS21-ABS-105-35-1.0105.4235.613.02.001.320.2015.42231231
EHS22-ABS-105-35-2.5105.2335.123.01.002.750.072.43226566
EHS23-ABS-100-50-1.099.6050.152.02.501.110.043.36238239
EHS24-ABS-100-50-2.099.8650.312.02.002.190.083.57236472
Specimena/bD D / σ σ ρ ρ N N N /N
(mm) (N/mm )(N/mm ) (kN)(kN)
EHS01-PLA1-100-50-3.02.0201141951.836.01.140.530.4426.5921.841.22
EHS02-PLA1-90-60-2.01.5135147050.037.31.160.550.4217.8813.751.30
EHS03-PLA1-90-60-2.01.5135145650.533.71.160.500.4316.2313.941.16
EHS04-PLA1-90-60-2.01.5135148349.534.01.170.500.4216.0813.501.19
EHS05-PLA1-100-50-3.02.0200144750.841.11.150.610.4329.6821.001.41
EHS06-PLA1-100-50-1.52.0200309623.725.41.690.380.238.765.341.64
EHS07-PLA1-105-35-2.03.0315350920.917.91.800.260.218.226.441.28
EHS08-PLA1-105-35-1.53.0315510014.412.92.170.190.154.103.331.23
EHS09-PLA2-90-60-1.01.5136244031.028.31.500.410.287.505.121.46
EHS10-PLA2-90-60-1.51.5135171044.242.31.260.610.3715.779.681.63
EHS11-PLA2-90-60-3.01.513590483.664.20.910.920.5944.3928.351.57
EHS12-PLA2-100-50-1.02.0200359721.018.11.820.260.204.873.811.28
EHS13-PLA2-100-50-2.02.0200179742.039.01.290.560.3620.7313.251.57
EHS14-PLA2-100-50-2.52.0200148450.949.11.170.710.4231.3418.691.68
EHS15-PLA2-105-35-1.03.0315507514.912.82.160.180.153.703.121.19
EHS16-PLA2-105-35-2.53.0315235232.128.11.470.400.2917.2312.231.41
EHS17-PLA2-105-35-3.03.0315199237.934.41.350.490.3324.7216.441.50
EHS18-ABS-100-50-1.02.019644139.445.282.000.140.171.531.890.80
EHS19-ABS-90-60-1.01.5136320213.0112.181.710.320.233.302.291.43
EHS20-ABS-90-60-1.51.5135205220.3117.131.360.450.327.155.061.39
EHS21-ABS-105-35-1.03.031264346.482.132.420.060.130.651.490.43
EHS22-ABS-105-35-2.53.0315312613.338.741.680.230.235.445.351.00
EHS23-ABS-100-50-1.02.019848468.606.792.100.180.161.801.611.11
EHS24-ABS-100-50-2.02.0198247516.8411.001.500.290.285.665.351.05
Material Batchσ
(N/mm )
 PLA batch 167.7
 PLA batch 269.6
 ABS37.8
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

McCann, F.; Rossi, F.; Sultan, S.D. Experimental Analysis and Design of 3D-Printed Polymer Elliptical Tubes in Compression. Appl. Sci. 2024 , 14 , 7673. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177673

McCann F, Rossi F, Sultan SD. Experimental Analysis and Design of 3D-Printed Polymer Elliptical Tubes in Compression. Applied Sciences . 2024; 14(17):7673. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177673

McCann, Finian, Federico Rossi, and Shahzada Danyal Sultan. 2024. "Experimental Analysis and Design of 3D-Printed Polymer Elliptical Tubes in Compression" Applied Sciences 14, no. 17: 7673. https://doi.org/10.3390/app14177673

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

Going beyond the comparison: toward experimental instructional design research with impact

  • Methodology
  • Published: 28 August 2024

Cite this article

experimental design application

  • Adam G. Gavarkovs 1 ,
  • Rashmi A. Kusurkar 2 , 3 , 4 ,
  • Kulamakan Kulasegaram 5 , 6 &
  • Ryan Brydges 6 , 7  

Explore all metrics

To design effective instruction, educators need to know what design strategies are generally effective and why these strategies work, based on the mechanisms through which they operate. Experimental comparison studies, which compare one instructional design against another, can generate much needed evidence in support of effective design strategies. However, experimental comparison studies are often not equipped to generate evidence regarding the mechanisms through which strategies operate. Therefore, simply conducting experimental comparison studies may not provide educators with all the information they need to design more effective instruction. To generate evidence for the what and the why of design strategies, we advocate for researchers to conduct experimental comparison studies that include mediation or moderation analyses, which can illuminate the mechanisms through which design strategies operate. The purpose of this article is to provide a conceptual overview of mediation and moderation analyses for researchers who conduct experimental comparison studies in instructional design. While these statistical techniques add complexity to study design and analysis, they hold great promise for providing educators with more powerful information upon which to base their instructional design decisions. Using two real-world examples from our own work, we describe the structure of mediation and moderation analyses, emphasizing the need to control for confounding even in the context of experimental studies. We also discuss the importance of using learning theories to help identify mediating or moderating variables to test.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

experimental design application

Research-Based Instructional Perspectives

experimental design application

To prove or improve, that is the question: the resurgence of comparative, confounded research between 2010 and 2019

experimental design application

Instructional Design Methods and Practice

Explore related subjects.

  • Artificial Intelligence

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

As an alternative to the regression approach, structural equation modelling (SEM) has gained popularity in the health professions education literature (Stoffels et al., 2023 ). SEM requires that a researcher make additional assumptions regarding the functional relationships between the covariates, the mediator(s), and the outcome(s) (VanderWeele, 2012 ). Though specifying these relationships can increase power, it comes with an increased risk of model misspecification (VanderWeele, 2012 ). Accordingly, we recommend that researchers beginning with experimental comparison studies involving a single mediator opt for using the regression-based approach with controls for mediator-outcome confounding (VanderWeele, 2012 ).

We did not actually analyze our data in the manner described below, for reasons described in our published manuscript. Here, we describe an alternative data analysis strategy for clarity.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173–1182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

Article   Google Scholar  

Bürkner, P. C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan . Journal of Statistical Software . https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the Self-Regulation of Behavior (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174794

Book   Google Scholar  

Cheung, J. J. H., & Kulasegaram, K. M. (2022). Beyond the tensions within transfer theories: Implications for adaptive expertise in the health professions. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 27 (5), 1293–1315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-022-10174-y

Cheung, J. J. H., Kulasegaram, K. M., Woods, N. N., & Brydges, R. (2019). Why Content and cognition matter: Integrating conceptual knowledge to support simulation-based procedural skills transfer. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 34 (6), 969–977. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-019-04959-y

Cheung, J. J. H., Kulasegaram, K. M., Woods, N. N., & Brydges, R. (2021). Making concepts material: A randomized trial exploring simulation as a medium to enhance cognitive integration and transfer of learning. Simulation in Healthcare: THe Journal of the Society for Simulation in Healthcare, 16 (6), 392–400. https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0000000000000543

Cheung, J. J. H., Kulasegaram, K. M., Woods, N. N., Moulton, C., Ringsted, C. V., & Brydges, R. (2018). Knowing How and Knowing Why: Testing the effect of instruction designed for cognitive integration on procedural skills transfer. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 23 (1), 61–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-017-9774-1

Cook, D. A. (2005). The research we still are not doing: An agenda for the study of computer-based learning. Academic Medicine, 80 (6), 541–548. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001888-200506000-00005

Cook, D. A. (2009). The failure of e-learning research to inform educational practice, and what we can do about it. Medical Teacher, 31 (2), 158–162. https://doi.org/10.1080/01421590802691393

Durik, A. M., Shechter, O. G., Noh, M., Rozek, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). What if I can’t? Success expectancies moderate the effects of utility value information on situational interest and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 39 (1), 104–118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9419-0

Ertmer, P. A., & Stepich, D. A. (2005). Instructional design expertise: How will we know it when we see it? Educational Technology, 45 (6), 38–43.

Google Scholar  

Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28 (4), 717–741. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9348-9

Friedman, C. P. (1994). The research we should be doing. Academic Medicine, 69 (6), 455–457. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001888-199406000-00005

Gavarkovs, A. G., Crukley, J., Miller, E., Kusurkar, R. A., Kulasegaram, K., & Brydges, R. (2023a). Effectiveness of life goal framing to motivate medical students during online learning: A randomized controlled trial. Perspectives on Medical Education, 12 (1), 444–454. https://doi.org/10.5334/pme.1017

Gavarkovs, A. G., Finan, E., Jensen, R. D., & Brydges, R. (2024). When I say … active learning. Medical Education . https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.15383

Gavarkovs, A. G., Kusurkar, R. A., & Brydges, R. (2023b). The purpose, adaptability, confidence, and engrossment model: A novel approach for supporting professional trainees’ motivation, engagement, and academic achievement. Frontiers in Education, 8 , 1036539. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2023.1036539

Hardré, P. L., Ge, X., & Thomas, M. K. (2005). Toward a model of development for instructional design expertise. Educational Technology, 45 (1), 53–57.

Hatano, G. & Inagaki, I. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In Child Development and Education in Japan (pp. 262–272). W. H. Freeman.

Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (3rd ed.). The Guilford Press.

Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19 (4), 509–539. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3

Kusurkar, R. A. (2023). Self-determination theory in health professions education research and practice. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The oxford handbook of self-determination theory (pp. 665–683). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197600047.013.33

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Kusurkar, R. A., Croiset, G., & Ten Cate, OTh. J. (2011). Twelve tips to stimulate intrinsic motivation in students through autonomy-supportive classroom teaching derived from Self-Determination Theory. Medical Teacher, 33 (12), 978–982. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2011.599896

Laidley, T. L., & Braddock, C. H. (2000). Role of adult learning theory in evaluating and designing strategies for teaching residents in ambulatory settings. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 5 (1), 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009863211233

Lawson, A. P., & Mayer, R. E. (2021). Benefits of writing an explanation during pauses in multimedia lessons. Educational Psychology Review, 33 (4), 1859–1885. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09594-w

Maheu-Cadotte, M.-A., Cossette, S., Dubé, V., Fontaine, G., Lavallée, A., Lavoie, P., Mailhot, T., & Deschênes, M.-F. (2021). Efficacy of serious games in healthcare professions education: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Simulation in Healthcare: THe Journal of the Society for Simulation in Healthcare, 16 (3), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0000000000000512

Mann, K. V. (2004). The role of educational theory in continuing medical education: Has it helped us? Journal of Continuing Education in the Health Professions, 24 (Supplement 1), S22–S30. https://doi.org/10.1002/chp.1340240505

Mayer, R. E. (2023). How to assess whether an instructional intervention has an effect on learning. Educational Psychology Review, 35 (2), 64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09783-9

Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., & Short, S. D. (2017). Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8 (4), 379–386. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617715068

Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2001). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference . Houghton Mifflin.

Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 845–851. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845

Stoffels, M., Torre, D. M., Sturgis, P., Koster, A. S., Westein, M. P. D., & Kusurkar, R. A. (2023). Steps and decisions involved when conducting structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis. Medical Teacher . https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2023.2263233

Tai, A.-S., Lin, S.-H., Chu, Y.-C., Yu, T., Puhan, M. A., & VanderWeele, T. (2023). Causal mediation analysis with multiple time-varying mediators. Epidemiology, 34 (1), 8–19. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0000000000001555

VanderWeele, T. J. (2012). Invited commentary: Structural equation models and epidemiologic analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 176 (7), 608–612. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kws213

VanderWeele, T. J. (2015). Explanation in causal inference: Methods for mediation and interaction . Oxford University Press.

VanderWeele, T. J. (2016). Mediation analysis: A practitioner’s guide. Annual Review of Public Health, 37 (1), 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032315-021402

VanderWeele, T. J., & Knol, M. J. (2014). A tutorial on interaction. Epidemiologic Methods . https://doi.org/10.1515/em-2013-0005

Woods, N. N., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2007). It all make sense: Biomedical knowledge, causal connections and memory in the novice diagnostician. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 12 (4), 405–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-006-9055-x

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, City Square East Tower, 555 W 12th Ave, Suite 200, Vancouver, BC, V5Z 3X7, Canada

Adam G. Gavarkovs

Research in Education, Amsterdam UMC Location Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, De Boelelaan 1118, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Rashmi A. Kusurkar

LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Amsterdam Public Health, Quality of Care, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Department of Family and Community Medicine, Temerty Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Kulamakan Kulasegaram

The Wilson Centre, University of Toronto/University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada

Kulamakan Kulasegaram & Ryan Brydges

Department of Medicine, Temerty Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Ryan Brydges

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

A.G. conceptualized the topic of the manuscript and wrote the first draft. R.K., K.K., and R.B. provided contributions to subsequent drafts of the manuscript. All authors reviewed the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam G. Gavarkovs .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Gavarkovs, A.G., Kusurkar, R.A., Kulasegaram, K. et al. Going beyond the comparison: toward experimental instructional design research with impact. Adv in Health Sci Educ (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-024-10365-9

Download citation

Received : 06 March 2024

Accepted : 05 August 2024

Published : 28 August 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-024-10365-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Randomized controlled trial
  • Quantitative data analysis
  • Learning theory
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. What Is Experimental Design?

    experimental design application

  2. Experimental Design Steps

    experimental design application

  3. Introduction to experimental design

    experimental design application

  4. (PDF) Experimental Design Research

    experimental design application

  5. Best What Is Experimental Design And Example Idea In 2022

    experimental design application

  6. Scientific method; it was a step by step process in which you make a hypthosis and have

    experimental design application

VIDEO

  1. Experimental Design Briefly Explain in Details || UGC-NET and WB SET EXAM || B.P.ED & M.P.ED

  2. Experimental design for the scientists…👀🧪 #elements #chemistry #science

  3. MyoMIDI demo (very early work in progress)

  4. DIY from Paper??? Abstract Home Decor idea

  5. Advanced Statistics and Experimental design Day 1

  6. 2024#Bangalore# wardrobe design#interior #modular #bengaluru # furniture# modular kitchen design

COMMENTS

  1. Guide to Experimental Design

    Table of contents. Step 1: Define your variables. Step 2: Write your hypothesis. Step 3: Design your experimental treatments. Step 4: Assign your subjects to treatment groups. Step 5: Measure your dependent variable. Other interesting articles. Frequently asked questions about experiments.

  2. Experimental Design Software

    Experimental design is the planning of an efficient, reliable, and accurate technical study. The range of application of experimental design principles is as broad as science and industry. One person may be planning a long-term agricultural experiment, while another may have eight hours to rectify a production problem.

  3. Experimental Design

    Applications of Experimental Design . Experimental design is a versatile research methodology that can be applied in many fields. Here are some applications of experimental design: Medical Research: Experimental design is commonly used to test new treatments or medications for various medical conditions. This includes clinical trials to ...

  4. Experimental Design: Definition and Types

    An experimental design is a detailed plan for collecting and using data to identify causal relationships. Through careful planning, the design of experiments allows your data collection efforts to have a reasonable chance of detecting effects and testing hypotheses that answer your research questions. An experiment is a data collection ...

  5. Desice

    Optimize your experiments and find the perfect settings for your factors. Desice is a tool for planning and analyzing your experiments. Designed to assist researchers and engineers get experimentation done right. In providing powerful tools we help you design the optimal experiment. Starting with an efficient and optimized experimental approach ...

  6. Guide to experimental research design

    Experimental design is a research method that enables researchers to assess the effect of multiple factors on an outcome.. You can determine the relationship between each of the variables by: Manipulating one or more independent variables (i.e., stimuli or treatments). Applying the changes to one or more dependent variables (i.e., test groups or outcomes)

  7. Design of experiments

    The design of experiments ( DOE or DOX ), also known as experiment design or experimental design, is the design of any task that aims to describe and explain the variation of information under conditions that are hypothesized to reflect the variation. The term is generally associated with experiments in which the design introduces conditions ...

  8. Design of Experiments Specialization [4 courses] (ASU)

    By the end of this course, you will be able to: Approach complex industrial and business research problems and address them through a rigorous, statistically sound experimental strategy. Use modern software to effectively plan experiments. Analyze the resulting data of an experiment, and communicate the results effectively to decision-makers.

  9. Design of Experiments software

    Providing experimental design, data analysis and optimisation for multi-factor and multi-response experiments is one of the strongest features of Effex. The OMARS designs accessible through the software have already helped industries save up to 40% of experimental effort and optimise problems with up to 18 different responses.

  10. Experimental Design Basics

    Applications from various fields will be illustrated throughout the course. Computer software packages (JMP, Design-Expert, Minitab) will be used to implement the methods presented and will be illustrated extensively. All experiments are designed experiments; some of them are poorly designed, and others are well-designed.

  11. Fundamentals of Experimental Design: Guidelines for Designing ...

    Four basic tenets or pillars of experimental design— replication, randomization, blocking, and size of experimental units— can be used creatively, intelligently, and consciously to solve both real and perceived problems in comparative experiments. ... Such a design is an application of the randomized complete block design (Cochran and Cox ...

  12. Principles of Experimental Design

    The (statistical) design of experiments provides the principles and methods for planning experiments and tailoring the data acquisition to an intended analysis.The design and analysis of an experiment are best considered as two aspects of the same enterprise: the goals of the analysis strongly inform an appropriate design, and the implemented design determines the possible analyses.

  13. Experimental Design: With Application in Management, Engineering, and

    About this book. This text introduces and provides instruction on the design and analysis of experiments for a broad audience. Formed by decades of teaching, consulting, and industrial experience in the Design of Experiments field, this new edition contains updated examples, exercises, and situations covering the science and engineering practice.

  14. A Quick Guide to Experimental Design

    A good experimental design requires a strong understanding of the system you are studying. There are five key steps in designing an experiment: Consider your variables and how they are related. Write a specific, testable hypothesis. Design experimental treatments to manipulate your independent variable.

  15. An Introduction to Experimental Design Research

    As discussed above, experimental design research encapsulates a wide range of research designs, sharing fundamental design conventions (see Part I, Chap. 3). Table 1.1 gives an overview of the basic types of experimental study, which are further elaborated with respect to design research in Chap. 12.

  16. Optimal experimental design

    Optimal design optimizes a numerical criterion, which typically relates to the variance or other statistically relevant properties of the design, and uses as input the number of runs, the factors ...

  17. Experimental design application and interpretation in pharmaceutical

    Abstract. This chapter provides a basic theoretical background on experimental design application and interpretation. Techniques described include screening designs, full and fractional factorial designs, Plackett-Burman designs, D-optimal designs, response surface methodology, central composite designs, Box-Behnken designs, mixture designs, etc.

  18. (PDF) Design of experiments application, concepts, examples: State of

    Abstract and Figures. Design of Experiments (DOE) is statistical tool deployed in various types of system, process and product design, development and optimization. It is multipurpose tool that ...

  19. Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

    In pre-experimental research design, either a group or various dependent groups are observed for the effect of the application of an independent variable which is presumed to cause change. It is the simplest form of experimental research design and is treated with no control group. ... It is the most accurate type of experimental design and may ...

  20. PDF Experimental Design and Its Application

    Experimental Design and Its Application Experimental Design is a vast concept. In this article, we will learn about: 1. The various terminologies required to build the concept ( experimental units, treatments, experimental error, blocks). 2. The three principles of Experimental Design i.e. Randomization, Replication, and Local Control 3.

  21. Experimental Design: Types, Examples & Methods

    Three types of experimental designs are commonly used: 1. Independent Measures. Independent measures design, also known as between-groups, is an experimental design where different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable. This means that each condition of the experiment includes a different group of participants.

  22. Experimental Design Research: Approaches, Perspectives, Applications

    This book presents a new, multidisciplinary perspective on and paradigm for integrative experimental design research. It addresses various perspectives on methods, analysis and overall research approach, and how they can be synthesized to advance understanding of design. It explores the foundations of experimental approaches and their utility ...

  23. Experimental Design: Definition, Principle, Steps, Types, Application

    Experimental design, also referred to as "design of experiment,"is a branch of applied statistics that deals with planning, conducting, analysing, and deciphering controlled tests.It is performed to evaluate the factors that control the value of a parameter or group of parameters. It is a powerful data collection and analysis tool that can be utilised in various experiments.

  24. Innovative applications in designed experiments

    For those of you who missed this episode, you can watch it on demand at your convenience, as well as see the slides from JMP presenters: Ryan Lekivetz, head of the Design of Experiments (DOE) and Reliability Development team, and Yeng Saanchi, Analytics Software Tester. They shared highlights of innovative applications in DOE and the first case ...

  25. Multi-Stage Optimal Experimental Design and Setup Strategies in Absence

    Optimal Experimental Design (OED) aims to maximize information about model parameters with minimal experiments. Methodically, OED is based on the principle of maximizing Fisher information. The calculation of an optimized test plan thereby requires a qualified estimate, i.e. a priori information, about the true value of the parameters to be estimated. This paper introduces a novel Multi-Stage ...

  26. Accurate design, simulation and implementation of AC/DC inductors for

    The mathematical foundations of the inductor's design are described in Section 2 with a holistic design methodology. The application in power electronics is described in Section 3. The performance of the electromagnetic design is explained in Section 4, where geometry, ... Before proceeding with the experimental prototype, ...

  27. Applied Sciences

    Local failure modes occurring in 3D-printed polymer elliptical section tubes in compression are investigated in the present study via a series of experiments, with the results compared to existing design proposals for slender steel analogues. Polylactic acid (PLA) and acrylonitrile butadiene styrene material specimens (ABS) have been printed in three orthogonal layering orientations, and ...

  28. Going beyond the comparison: toward experimental instructional design

    To design effective instruction, educators need to know what design strategies are generally effective and why these strategies work, based on the mechanisms through which they operate. Experimental comparison studies, which compare one instructional design against another, can generate much needed evidence in support of effective design strategies. However, experimental comparison studies are ...